Augmenting hippocampal–prefrontal neuronal synchrony during sleep enhances memory consolidation in humans

Augmenting hippocampal–prefrontal neuronal synchrony during sleep enhances memory consolidation in humans


Play all audios:


ABSTRACT Memory consolidation during sleep is thought to depend on the coordinated interplay between cortical slow waves, thalamocortical sleep spindles and hippocampal ripples, but direct


evidence is lacking. Here, we implemented real-time closed-loop deep brain stimulation in human prefrontal cortex during sleep and tested its effects on sleep electrophysiology and on


overnight consolidation of declarative memory. Synchronizing the stimulation to the active phases of endogenous slow waves in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) enhanced sleep spindles, boosted


locking of brain-wide neural spiking activity to MTL slow waves, and improved coupling between MTL ripples and thalamocortical oscillations. Furthermore, synchronized stimulation enhanced


the accuracy of recognition memory. By contrast, identical stimulation without this precise time-locking was not associated with, and sometimes even degraded, these electrophysiological and


behavioral effects. Notably, individual changes in memory accuracy were highly correlated with electrophysiological effects. Our results indicate that hippocampo–thalamocortical


synchronization during sleep causally supports human memory consolidation. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL SIGNATURES UNDERLYING VARIABILITY IN HUMAN MEMORY


CONSOLIDATION Article Open access 12 March 2025 COORDINATED NREM SLEEP OSCILLATIONS AMONG HIPPOCAMPAL SUBFIELDS MODULATE SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN HUMANS Article Open access 01 October 2024


RESPIRATION MODULATES SLEEP OSCILLATIONS AND MEMORY REACTIVATION IN HUMANS Article Open access 18 December 2023 MAIN For decades, it has been demonstrated that sleep plays an important role


in long-term memory consolidation1,2,3,4. Systems-level memory consolidation theory posits that the initial phase of the formation of a declarative memory trace (that is, memories that are


accessible to conscious recollection, such as memory for facts and events5) is primarily supported by the hippocampus. Over time, declarative memory representations become increasingly


dependent upon the neocortex (a ‘two-stage’ model)4,6,7. A central notion in this model is that embedding novel information in the neocortex relies on offline reactivation of acquired


information by the hippocampus around ripple events, primarily during slow-wave sleep4,8. Ripples (brief oscillatory events with frequencies of ~80–120 Hz in humans) occur in and around the


hippocampus in the MTL, as well as in the neocortex9,10. Extensive research in rodents has established the role of hippocampal ripples in memory consolidation11 and their widespread impact


on neocortical activities during sleep12,13. Slow waves (<4 Hz) reflect regionally synchronous alternations between active states of membrane depolarization and spiking, and inactive


states of hyperpolarization and neuronal quiescence14,15. According to the active system consolidation framework, slow-wave active states serve as a temporal frame for offline consolidation


via synchronization of thalamocortical sleep spindles (9–16 Hz) and ripple oscillations4,16,17. Thus, sleep-dependent memory consolidation is believed to be mediated by coordinated


oscillations across hippocampus, thalamus and neocortex. Most evidence supporting this theory stems from correlative studies, performed either noninvasively in humans1,4,18,19 or with


neuronal recordings in rodents20,21,22,23,24. Causal manipulations of ripple activities in rodents established that ripples are necessary for optimal memory consolidation25,26,27. To date,


only a few studies have provided causal support for the role of coordinated hippocampal–neocortical interactions during sleep in mediating memory consolidation20,21. In humans, there is


evidence highlighting extensive hippocampal–neocortical interactions during sleep13,28,29, but direct causal evidence linking these interactions with memory consolidation is currently


missing30. Here, we designed a closed-loop stimulation protocol to dynamically enhance the temporal coupling between MTL ripples, neocortical slow waves and thalamocortical spindles during


non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep to directly test the role of their temporal coupling in overnight consolidation of declarative memory. RESULTS Neurosurgical patients with


pharmacoresistant epilepsy who were implanted with intracranial depth electrodes for clinical reasons (_n_ = 18, ages 19–47 years, all fluent English speakers; Supplementary Table 1)


provided written informed consent before participation in a study approved by the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) Institutional Review Board. The unique intracranial clinical


setup allowed simultaneous recordings of intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG) and single-neuron activity in the MTL and distant neocortical sites. Electrophysiology was complemented by


cognitive assessment, whereby participants were tested during two experimental nights (order counterbalanced): an intervention night and an undisturbed night (Fig. 1a and Supplementary


Table 2). This within-participant design helped control for individual variability in clinical and memory profiles31. On the intervention night, real-time closed-loop (RTCL) stimulation was


performed intermittently in 5-min blocks for a total of ~90 min during early NREM sleep (Methods, Fig. 1b, Extended Data Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 3). One iEEG electrode in the MTL


served as a synchronization probe for determining the timing of closed-loop control, while a second neocortical iEEG electrode served as the stimulation site (typically, in orbitofrontal


cortex white matter (15 of 19 stimulation nights); Fig. 1c,d and Extended Data Figs. 2 and 3). Slow-wave activity in the MTL probe was monitored and analyzed in real time to trigger brief


(50 ms) high-frequency (100 Hz) electrical stimulation events in the neocortical stimulation site roughly once every 4 s (Methods). The closed-loop intervention had two modes of operation


(Methods and Extended Data Fig. 3)—either (i) ‘synchronized (sync) stimulation’ (Fig. 1c) or (ii) ‘mixed-phase stimulation’—which were applied in two separate groups of participants.


Sync-stimulation involved neocortical stimulation that was time-locked to the MTL slow-wave active phase, aimed at synchronizing MTL with thalamocortical activities. During these active


phases, corresponding to the iEEG negative peak (Fig. 1c)32, ripples occur more frequently and their prevalence is believed to be key for hippocampal–cortical communication20,28,33,34.


During mixed-phase stimulation, performed in a separate group of participants, identical neocortical stimulations were applied but their timing was without regard to the MTL slow-wave phase


(Methods and Extended Data Fig. 3). Given our previous work on local sleep oscillations, where MTL slow waves can be phase-shifted or even entirely independent from neocortical slow


waves32,35,36, we hypothesized that sync-stimulation would be key in increasing hippocampo–neocortical coupling and that stimulating white-matter electrodes during sleep would allow local


low-amplitude stimulation to affect wide territories36,37. SYNCHRONIZED STIMULATION IMPROVED MEMORY ACCURACY To assess the effects of the intervention on overnight memory consolidation,


participants performed a visual paired-association task before sleep, learning 25 pairings between photos of famous people and animals (contextualized as ‘pet owners’ and their pets) on the


evening before each experimental night (Fig. 1e and Methods). A different set of images was used each night. Two different measures assessed two separate aspects of memory performance.


First, we evaluated recognition memory via responses to learned images and a set of lures, quantifying recognition memory accuracy as the difference between hit rate and false-detection rate


(Methods). Second, we evaluated the successful pairing (association) of each person to their associated animal. Performance on these two measures was assessed four times for each


participant (two time points × two nights), without any feedback on responses: in the evening (several minutes following learning) and the morning (following overnight sleep), separately


around an intervention night and an undisturbed sleep night (Fig. 1a). Twelve participants completed this full cognitive testing suite, and six additional participants were only included in


neurophysiological analyses (Methods and Supplementary Tables 2 and 3). In 6/6 participants receiving sync-stimulation in prefrontal cortex white matter (Fig. 1f,g), recognition memory


accuracy following the intervention night was superior to that following undisturbed sleep (*_P_ = 0.01 based on a binomial probability distribution; Methods and Extended Data Fig. 4c–e).


Mixed results were observed for sync-stimulation delivered in other posterior neocortical regions (Fig. 1f,g; _n_ = 3), and a trend for degraded performance was observed for participants who


received mixed-phase stimulation (Fig. 1f,g; _n_ = 3). Sync-stimulation did not reliably affect the pairing (association) accuracy (Extended Data Fig. 4a,b). We did not find significant


correlation between baseline recognition memory accuracy on the first evening test and intervention efficacy (Spearman correlation; _n_ = 9 sync-stimulation participants, ρ = 0.04, _P_ = 


0.9), suggesting memory improvements were not unique to participants with lower performance. Sync-stimulation did not significantly alter reaction times during memory recall compared with


undistributed sleep (Wilcoxon rank-sum test: _P_ = 0.65; Extended Data Fig. 4f). Conversely, overnight decrease in reaction times, representing an improvement in a visual psychomotor


vigilance task (PVT; Methods38) was significantly lower following sleep with sync-stimulation as compared with undisturbed sleep (Extended Data Fig. 4g), indicating that memory improvement


does not reflect an across-the-board improvement in behavioral performance. Therefore, memory accuracy improvement associated with sync-stimulation likely reflects an enhanced stabilizing


effect of sleep to reduce forgetting3,4. CORRELATED ENHANCEMENT OF SLEEP SPINDLES AND MEMORY ACCURACY To test whether changes in sleep electrophysiology underlie the observed behavioral


changes, we first examined how stimulation modulates slow waves and spindles, known to be tightly linked to memory consolidation21,39,40,41,42. We used two complementary analysis approaches,


in the power domain and in the time domain. In both analyses, a within-session approach compared the modulation in every iEEG contact to its session-specific baseline (see Methods for


baseline selection). We evaluated any measure of interest separately for sync-stimulation or mixed-phase stimulation modes (Methods) to test how the temporal accuracy of stimulation affects


sleep electrophysiology. First, spectral analysis was used to test whether time–frequency representations (spectrograms) of iEEG signals in multiple brain areas reveal changes in spindle


power in an a priori-defined frequency range of 9–16 Hz following stimulation events (Methods). We used a protocol in which short stimulation bursts (50 ms) were delivered, spaced by more


than 4 s, during 5-min stimulation blocks interleaved with 5-min pause blocks, during which no stimulation was provided (Methods and Fig. 1b). We first studied the immediate effects of


stimulations (as observed within stimulation blocks), during 3-s periods following single stimulation bursts. Sync-stimulation bursts immediately increased sigma (spindle) power relative to


a 1-s pre-stimulation baseline across the brain (Fig. 2a and Methods). Importantly, this increase went above and beyond the expected tendency of spindle power to increase around slow-wave


active states35,43 because it was significantly greater than that found in sham-stimulation moments during intermittent ‘pause’ blocks that had identical delays from MTL slow-wave peaks


(Methods; Fig. 2a(ii): Wilcoxon signed-rank test found a significant increase in spindle-band for sync-stimulation contacts; _n_ = 565 iEEG contacts from MTL and neocortical sites, _P_ < 


10−30). Conversely, we did not find any immediate change in spindle power in the mixed-phase stimulation group (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, _n_ = 215, _P_ = 0.73). Nor did we find an increase


in post-stimulation power when examining a control frequency range (20–27 Hz). Next, we compared the immediate effects of sync-stimulation and mixed-phase stimulation modes on slow waves


and spindles in the time domain, by selectively identifying individual slow-wave and spindle events on each iEEG contact, according to established detection algorithms35,44 (Fig. 2b and


Methods). The probability of slow-wave events was reduced for both sync-stimulation and mixed-phase conditions immediately following stimulation (Extended Data Fig. 5a). However, a


significant increase in spindle detection probability was observed immediately following sync-stimulation (in the 3-s inter-stimulus interval) compared to sham-stimulation time points with


identical delays from MTL slow-wave peaks (Fig. 2c(i); Wilcoxon signed-rank test, _P_ < 10−4). Conversely, in the mixed-phase condition, immediate iEEG spindle detection probability was


significantly decreased (Fig. 2c(ii); _P_ < 10−8, Extended Data Fig. 5b). Critically, when considering all participants (both sync-stimulation and mixed-phase stimulation), behavioral


changes in recognition memory accuracy were highly correlated with the degree to which stimulation affected immediate spindle occurrence (Fig. 2c(iii); Spearman correlation ρ = 0.69, _P_ = 


0.013; _n_ = 12 participants). A high correlation coefficient between immediate spindle increase and memory increase was also observed when focusing on the smaller group of nine participants


receiving sync-stimulation, but this did not reach statistical significance (Fig. 2c(iii); ρ = 0.53, _P_ = 0.148). Next, we also tested for prolonged effects of stimulation occurring beyond


the 5-min stimulation blocks, by comparing the rates of iEEG sleep oscillations in the 1-min following each stimulation block with the 1 min at the end of each ‘pause’ block, using a


normalized pre/post index (Methods). We found that sync-stimulation led to prolonged enhancement of spindle rate, whereas mixed-phase stimulation led to prolonged decrease in spindle rate


(Fig. 2d and Extended Data Fig. 5e; Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing sync-stim and mixed-phase spindle enhancement index distributions; _P_ < 10−8). Interestingly, prolonged spindle rate


increase was observed in both hemispheres across wide cortical territories including MTL and neocortical electrodes (Fig. 2d and Extended Data Fig. 5d–i; Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing


neocortical and MTL spindle enhancement index distributions, _P_ = 0.13; Methods). While slow-wave rates following sync-stimulation were comparable before and after stimulation blocks,


mixed-phase stimulation led to a significant reduction in slow-wave occurrence when assessing prolonged effects (Fig. 2d and Extended Data Fig. 5d; Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing sync-stim


and mixed-phase slow-wave enhancement index distributions, _P_ < 10−7). Together, analysis of slow waves and sleep spindles revealed that deep brain stimulation (DBS) that is


synchronized to MTL slow-wave active phases leads to robust and widespread effects that persist beyond stimulation blocks, particularly enhancing spindles in a manner correlated with memory


benefits. STIMULATION INCREASED SPIKE PHASE-LOCKING TO MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE SLOW WAVES Next, we assessed the effects of sync-stimulation on phase locking of spiking in neural units to MTL.


We quantified how sync-stimulation affected the timing of neuronal action potential discharges recorded on individual microwires (Extended Data Fig. 6a(i)) across multiple brain regions,


with respect to MTL iEEG slow-wave phase (0.5–4 Hz; this signal was used for stimulation timing; Supplementary Table 4). For each neuronal unit (spike-sorted offline; Methods), we calculated


the phase of each spike relative to the MTL slow wave and fitted each distribution with a cosine function to evaluate the depth of phase locking and preferred firing phase (Methods and


Extended Data Fig. 6b; _n_ = 325 neuronal units in eight sync-stimulation participants with microwire recordings). First, we evaluated the changes in neuronal firing by comparing


phase-locking depth during stimulation blocks (apart from intervals around stimulation bursts) and pause blocks to several baseline options (Methods, Extended Data Fig. 6c,d and Fig. 3a). To


include as many action potential events in this analysis—even for low firing rate units—we focused on long intervals, combining all 5-min stimulation blocks (‘stim’) and comparing them with


10–15 min periods of baseline activity before the first stimulation block (‘PRE’). We found that sync-stimulation increased the proportion of neuronal units outside the MTL with significant


phase locking to MTL iEEG active states from 34% to 50% (Fig. 3b), but this percentage was only modestly altered for MTL units (46.5% to 50.5%; Fig. 3b). Next, as examined for sleep


spindles, we investigated potential prolonged effects in the 1-min intervals beyond stimulation blocks to better understand the dynamics and regional variability of phase-locking changes


(Methods). In the first minute following every stimulation block, the percentage of phase-locked cells returned to baseline (34% for units outside MTL) but units increased the phase-locking


depth relative to baseline, and this was specific to neural units outside the MTL where observed effects were significant (Fig. 3c, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; units outside the MTL: _P_ = 


0.007/_n_ = 47; units in the MTL: _P_ = 0.9/_n_ = 26, but Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing populations _P_ = 0.1). Even when restricting the data to compare only the first minute following


each stimulation block with the last minute of the previous pause block, we found significantly higher depth of phase locking immediately after sync-stimulation (Fig. 3d; Wilcoxon


signed-rank test when aggregating all neural units—_P_ < 10−3). This result represents a prolonged effect with dynamics similar to the decay profile we observed for spindles and slow


waves, decreasing within minutes. Prolonged effects were validated via two shuffling procedures (Methods)—by verifying that real data significantly differed from a shuffled distribution with


randomly assigned condition labels (Extended Data Fig. 6c; Wilcoxon rank-sum test, _n_ = 57 neural units, _P_ = 0.02), and by verifying that phase locking is not dependent on possible


changes in mean firing rates (Extended Data Fig. 6c; Kolmogorov–Smirnov two-sample test, _P_ = 0.7). The presence of robust prolonged effects allowed us to confidently interpret changes as


true differences in synchronization rather than potential contamination by stimulation artifacts. Together, we found that sync-stimulation led to prolonged increases in phase locking of


neuronal spiking activity, particularly in distant regions, to MTL slow waves, which decayed within minutes. INCREASED RIPPLE-SLOW WAVE-SPINDLE COUPLING CORRELATES WITH MEMORY Next, we


focused on the coordination between hippocampal ripples and thalamocortical slow waves and spindles, previously suggested to mediate sleep-dependent memory consolidation. To this end, we


focused on a subset of 16 participants where hippocampal and prefrontal activities were simultaneously monitored. Ripples (Fig. 4a; _n_ = 7,172 events) were detected in MTL iEEG electrodes


during pre-stimulation sleep using an automated algorithm44 (Methods), using bipolar referencing to minimize volume conduction effects. Most events were detected in the hippocampus (CA1,


CA3/DG and subiculum), but also in adjacent entorhinal cortex and in the parahippocampal gyrus (see Extended Data Fig. 7 for breakdown to different MTL sub-areas), where ripples have been


previously reported28,45,46,47. An extensive inspection of detected ripple events, along with their narrow-band frequency profile around 80–120 Hz, attests to largely successful separation


from pathological high-frequency MTL oscillations and interictal epileptiform discharges (IEDs), which exhibited a distinct spectral profile with wide-band higher-frequency pathological


activity (Extended Data Fig. 8a). Ripple detection probability decreased immediately following stimulation bursts (in 3-s inter-stimulus intervals) relative to sham moments in


stimulation-free intervals, in both sync-stimulation and mixed-phase stimulation (Extended Data Fig. 9a and Methods; Wilcoxon signed-rank test: _P_ = 0.004/_n_ = 18 and _P_ = 0.0.01/_n_ = 8


for sync-stim and mixed-phase stim iEEG contacts, respectively, no significant difference between groups). However, this reduction did not extend beyond stimulation blocks, as we did not


find a prolonged change in ripple rate when focusing on the first 1-min intervals of pause periods as in other analyses (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 9b; Wilcoxon signed-rank test yielded


nonsignificant _P_ values for both stimulation modes and no significant difference was found between groups). Although we did not find an increase in the incidence of ripple events, we asked


whether synchronizing stimulation induced an increase in temporal coupling between hippocampal ripples and oscillations outside the MTL, namely neocortical slow waves and thalamocortical


spindles. We evaluated the coupling incidence in cross-brain electrode pairs such that one electrode was placed in MTL and exhibited ripples during pre-stimulation sleep and the other was


located in the neocortex with robust spindle activity (all neocortical contacts except five were located in the orbitofrontal cortex; Fig. 4b and Methods). We further focused on fast (>11


 Hz) sleep spindles (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 10), because these are preferentially associated with memory consolidation42,48, are more synchronized with slow-wave active phases48, and


are associated with hippocampus activation49, even though they are not as prevalent in prefrontal cortex35. We found that sync-stimulation, but not mixed-phase stimulation, enhanced the


temporal coupling between MTL ripples and neocortical slow waves in the 1-min following every stimulation block, relative to 1 min preceding stimulation blocks (prolonged effects as in other


electrophysiology analyses; Methods and Fig. 4c, right-tailed Wilcoxon sign-rank test: **_P_ < 10−3, _n_ = 25 pairs, for sync-stimulation; _P_ > 0.5, _n_ = 13 pairs for mixed-phase


stimulation; rank-sum Wilcoxon test comparing distributions of changes in both stimulation modes, _P_ < 0.05; Extended Data Fig. 9d). Furthermore, using a more stringent triple


co-occurrence criterion of cortical slow waves, thalamocortical sleep spindles and MTL ripples (Methods), we also found that sync-stimulation increased co-occurrence of hippocampal and


thalamocortical sleep oscillations (Fig. 4d; right-tailed Wilcoxon sign-rank test: *_P_ = 0.03/_n_ = 5 and _P_ > 0.5/_n_ = 7, for sync-stimulation and mixed-stimulation groups,


respectively, rank-sum Wilcoxon test comparing distributions of changes in both stimulation modes: _P_ = 0.01; Extended Data Fig. 9e). Finally, to test our initial hypothesis, that is,


whether increased co-occurrence of hippocampal and thalamocortical sleep oscillations mediates overnight memory consolidation, we examined the relationship between electrophysiology and


memory (Fig. 4e). We observed a robust correlation between recognition memory accuracy change and increase in co-occurrence of sleep oscillations—MTL ripples and neocortical slow waves (_n_ 


= 30 MTL-cortical electrode pairs in eight participants; Spearman correlation calculated for all pairs ρ = 0.8, _P_ = 0.007; Methods). When focusing only on smaller subsets of data (for


example, only sync-stimulation data, or triple co-occurrence of ripples, spindles and slow waves; Fig. 4e), we observed positive correlations that did not reach significance (ρ = 0.8, _P_ = 


0.1, _n_ = 18 pairs; and ρ = 0.7, _P_ = 0.2, _n_ = 12 pairs, respectively). Together, our results support the notion that improving the temporal coupling between human MTL ripples and


thalamocortical sleep oscillations is key for overnight memory consolidation. DISCUSSION Dynamically modulating the coupling between MTL ripples, cortical slow waves and thalamocortical


spindles in human sleep via intracranial DBS synchronized in real time with MTL slow-wave active periods reveals a robust relation between MTL–neocortical coupling and overnight


consolidation of recognition memory. In addition, we show that sync-stimulation enhances spindle activity and improves phase locking of brain-wide neuronal spiking activity to MTL slow


waves. Importantly, across all participants receiving either sync-stimulation or mixed-phase stimulation, individual overnight changes in memory accuracy are tightly correlated with these


electrophysiological effects. The efficacy of the closed-loop intervention was observed when applying temporally precise stimulation relative to local MTL activity—made possible by its


real-time monitoring—but not with stimulation that was not precisely timed. Our data demonstrate that multiple neocortical areas distant from the stimulation focus, even in the contralateral


hemisphere, undergo similar coupling to the MTL following prefrontal sync-stimulation, as does the prefrontal cortex adjacent to the stimulation site. Wide-brain prolonged effects are


likely made possible by the unique state of brain activity during sleep that allows local, low-amplitude intracranial stimulation to propagate effectively across wide cortical territories50.


While we establish that prefrontal white-matter DBS leads to robust electrophysiological and memory effects, stimulation in other neocortical sites may also lead to similar effects.


Nevertheless, the effects observed with our stimulation sites are consistent both with the efficacy of white-matter stimulation37,51,52,53,54 and with the known role of MTL–prefrontal


interactions in memory20,22,33,34. Paired-associate learning (PAL) paradigms have been widely used in studies of human sleep and declarative memory ever since the early pioneering studies by


Jenkins and Dallenbach1. Typically, word-pair associations are used to reveal that sleep is associated with reduced forgetting compared with wake intervals39,42. In this study, we used a


visual paired-associate learning (vPAL) task55 of image associations, using a naturalistic approach suited for clinical settings in which learning took place during a one-shot viewing


session. We tested participants on their memory for the associations, as well as introducing ‘lure’ images to test for recognition memory. The same lures were used in evening and morning


testing, so the morning test can be described as a source recognition test56 requiring participants to distinguish learned images from lures they have seen before but in a different context.


With this task, we found that our intervention improved recognition memory accuracy, measured by the difference between correct recollection (hit rate) and false lure acceptance (false


alarms; Fig. 1g and Extended Data Fig. 4c), whereas pairing performance was only modestly increased (Extended Data Fig. 4a,b). Our data suggest that the dominant factor in the performance


increase associated with sync-stimulation was a reduction in falsely tagging lures (Extended Data Fig. 4e), in line with previous studies that pointed to the effect of sleep on minimizing


false memory formation57,58,59. The fact that sleep specifically benefited recognition accuracy in this memory task was also reported in a separate cohort of healthy participants55. Several


factors may contribute to the difference between our results, where association performance was not significantly impacted, and previous PAL-based studies. Such factors include lower initial


memory strength related to the lack of rehearsal during the learning phase, the use of an immediate memory retrieval that may constitute a re-consolidation step, and the type of stimuli


used (easily recognizable images of celebrities versus words; see also Discussion in ref. 55). Additional studies are needed to determine how sleep affects consolidation of memory accuracy


and association. The participants in the current study were individuals with medically refractory epilepsy. Their pathology and medication can affect sleep in multiple dimensions (reviewed


in ref. 60). Briefly, refractory temporal lobe epilepsy is associated with excessive daytime sleepiness and changes to sleep architecture61 when nocturnal seizures may occur. Anti-epileptic


drugs reduce the amount of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep62, and changes in the dynamics and power of slow waves and sleep spindles have been observed in this population61,63. In addition,


IEDs occur preferentially during NREM sleep64, whereby elevated neuronal synchrony within thalamocortical networks facilitates the spread of focal IEDs to distant brain areas65. While we


acknowledge that these aspects represent potential confounding factors, several observations suggest that these issues do not likely play a major role in our findings. First, anticipating


variability in participant age, cognitive abilities, epilepsy profile and medication regime (Supplementary Table 2), we designed our cognitive paradigm as a ‘within-participant’ design


(rather than a within-group comparison) thereby minimizing the contributions of these factors. Second, we observed similar behavioral and electrophysiology results despite variability in


clinical profiles (Supplementary Table 1) and medication regimes (Supplementary Table 2), arguing against a major role. Third, while previous studies suggest that IEDs may impair memory by


interfering with physiological hippocampal–cortical coupling66, we did not observe an association between recognition memory accuracy and the degree to which stimulation affected IEDs


(Methods and Extended Data Fig. 8b), arguing against the possibility that memory benefits reported here are driven by IED reduction. Additional studies are needed to generalize findings


based on this participant population to the general population. Where do our results stand in relation to previous literature on boosting memory via closed-loop manipulations and sleep


interventions? In humans, both open-loop and closed-loop intracranial electrical stimulation during the encoding phase have been previously reported in awake participants—by our laboratory


and others—to benefit memory performance51,67,68,69. The current study highlights an alternative approach of interventions during offline memory consolidation, where sleep offers a


privileged window of opportunity70. Previous studies have used closed-loop acoustic stimulation to demonstrate enhancement of neocortical slow oscillations and sleep spindles that improves


memory40,71,72, although some studies could not replicate memory effects despite strong effects on slow waves and spindles73,74. One unresolved issue in all these studies is what impact the


stimulation had on hippocampal ripples. In our data, we find that even though stimulation did not increase the number of ripples, the temporal co-occurrence of neocortical slow waves and


hippocampal ripples is a critical predictor of memory accuracy. Possibly, the degree to which previous manipulations modulated this co-occurrence may account for the discrepancies observed


in the behavioral results but, unfortunately, hippocampal ripples were not measured in those studies due to the absence of deep recording electrodes. Thus, additional studies will be


required to further address the degree to which modulation of hippocampal ripples per se is necessary to induce memory benefits via stimulation. Several features of our experimental design


make it unique among previous reports. These include: (i) using intracranial electrical stimulation in frontal lobe white matter aiming to influence prefrontal–hippocampal pathways34; (ii)


timing the stimulations in real time based on MTL slow-wave active periods, which are not necessarily in sync with slow-wave up-states as recorded on the scalp32; this was made possible here


because of our access to deep brain iEEG signals; (iii) we developed a brief, high-frequency stimulation scheme (Methods); (iv) our setup allowed simultaneous iEEG recordings from deep


brain structures to evaluate coupling between MTL ripples to slow wave and spindle events; and (v) our ability to assess the effects of intervention on neuronal spiking activities. Our


results suggest that timing the interventions to moments of MTL active periods is key to achieving the memory-enhancing effects given the difference between sync stimulation and mixed-phase


control stimulation. This result is in agreement both with rodent studies, pointing to cross-brain synchrony during slow-wave active states as an important factor supporting successful


learning and memory consolidation20,21,24 and with studies in humans, supporting the idea that these states offer a privileged window for interventions targeting memory


consolidation36,40,75. However, we do not claim that the precise timing we have chosen in this report is necessarily optimal. Additional studies should test whether synchrony could be


further enhanced by refinement of the stimulation timing. To conclude, using a rare opportunity to perform an active intracranial intervention during natural sleep and while recording


detailed iEEG signals and single-unit spiking from humans, we found a tight correlation between electrophysiological signatures of NREM sleep and overnight memory consolidation. Our results


support present models of systems-level consolidation, whereby functional coupling between hippocampal ripples and thalamocortical sleep spindles and slow waves mediates fine-tuned


communication between the human hippocampus and neocortex during sleep. Finally, the present study suggests an approach to benefit memory consolidation during the privileged period of sleep,


that may be beneficial in future development of closed-loop clinical DBS devices for memory disorders and dementia37. METHODS PARTICIPANTS Eighteen participants with pharmacologically


intractable epilepsy (11 women, 7 men, based on self-reported gender identity; Supplementary Table 1) who met clinical criteria for depth electrode placement at UCLA for identification of


seizure foci and preparation for surgical treatment76. Participants were not compensated for participation in the study. The UCLA Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol. All


participants provided written consent to participate in the study. Electrode location was based solely on clinical criteria. Predetermined clinical criteria guided placement of 9–14


Behnke–Fried electrodes (Adtech Medical, Racine WI) in each individual. Electrodes were implanted stereotactically with the aid of digital subtraction angiography or computed tomography (CT)


angiography as well as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)76. Each Behnke–Fried macro–micro depth electrode contained at least seven macroelectrode contacts (1.5 mm wide) spaced 1.5–3.5 mm


apart along the shaft, and a Behnke–Fried inner platinum-iridium microwire bundle (California Fine Wire)76 (Extended Data Fig. 6a(i)). All surgeries were performed by I.F. Sixteen


participants were tested in two experimental sessions, as detailed below (Supplementary Table 2), while two participants underwent mixed-phase stimulation on intervention nights (without a


night of undisturbed sleep), designed to serve as a control for physiological effects. Each participant’s drug regimen at the time of recording is listed in Supplementary Table 2.


EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN Participants were tested in two experimental sessions: an intervention condition (sleep with RTCL stimulation) and a control condition (undisturbed sleep), with the order


of conditions and test versions (image pairs), counterbalanced across participants (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Table 2). The participants’ two experimental conditions were separated by an


interval of 1–5 d (Supplementary Table 2). On the day of each experimental session, participants did not take any naps nor drank coffee in the 6 h preceding the experiment. In each


condition, participants (i) first performed a declarative memory task (image-pair associates, below) between 20:00 and 22:30 (learning phase), (ii) were tested on their memory (after a short


break) following learning, (iii) went to sleep; in stimulation nights, RTCL stimulation started after at least 30 min of consolidated sleep when online polysomnography indicated unequivocal


NREM sleep and was discontinued 90–140 min later, and (iv) completed a memory recall examination session (retrieval phase) in the morning 30–60 min after awakening (Supplementary Table 2).


Of 16 participants tested on two experimental nights, three exhibited poor memory accuracy scores (recognition memory accuracy < 0.1 in one of the tests, suggesting low attention or


misunderstanding of the task, marked with an asterisk in Supplementary Table 2) and were excluded from further memory accuracy analysis (but included in neurophysiological analysis). One


additional participant underwent cognitive testing but had a very low number of stimulations delivered (<100) and was excluded altogether from the cognitive cohort (Supplementary Table


2). COGNITIVE TESTING AND OVERNIGHT MEMORY PARADIGM Paired associative learning (PAL) has previously shown to be sensitive to the effect of sleep1,40,77,78,79. We adapted the task for


participants to make it relatively short (20 min for the learning phase, 5–10 min for the retrieval phase), and based on visual stimuli (vPAL; see also ref. 55). Twenty-five color images of


famous people were paired with 25 different animals (their ‘pets’). Image pairs were presented on a laptop computer at the participant’s bedside. Participants studied each image pair for 2 s


followed by 2 s of fixation and were asked to memorize the pair and to name the animal’s type. After viewing each pair once, participants were given a short break in which the rest of the


experiment was explained. Next, single images of people were presented for 2 s (25 learned images were mixed with 15 novel (‘lure’) images of famous people). First, participants were asked


whether the person was a previously learned pet owner, without any feedback on their answer. Next, if they had identified a person as a ‘pet owner’, they were asked what kind of animal they


owned (pairing). For each image, we determined whether it was correctly identified as new (lure) or old, the reaction time of that response, and whether the associated pet was correctly


identified (when applicable). After an overnight sleep period (Supplementary Table 2; average delay ± standard deviation between the first and second tests, 10.7 ± 1.3 h on intervention


night and 10.3 h ± 2.7 h on undisturbed nights, respectively), participants were retested (same 40 images as in the evening test, in a scrambled order) without feedback, and behavioral


measures were compared for pre-sleep and post-sleep tests. Recognition accuracy for each test was defined as: $$\mathrm{Accuracy} =


\frac{{N_{\mathrm{correct}}\;{\mathrm{recognition}}}}{{25}} - \frac{{N_{\mathrm{false}}\;{\mathrm{recognition}}}}{{15}}$$ Overnight memory change was quantified as: $$\Delta


{\mathrm{Accuracy}} = {\mathrm{Accuracy}_{morning}} - {\mathrm{Accuracy}_{evening}}$$ The efficacy of RTCL stimulation was evaluated by comparing overnight performance changes:


$$\begin{array}{l}\mathrm{Intervention}\;\mathrm{efficacy}\\ = {{\Delta }}\mathrm{Accuracy}_{\mathrm{RTCL}\;\mathrm{intervention}\;\mathrm{night}} - {{\Delta


}}\mathrm{Accuracy}_{\mathrm{undisturbed}\;\mathrm{sleep}\;\mathrm{night}}\end{array}$$ This within-participant comparison was performed to distill the effects of the intervention (sleep


with RTCL stimulation compared to undisturbed sleep), independent of the expected inter-participant variability in baseline long-term memory performance31. If the intervention had no


consistent effect, we would expect by chance that the intervention night would show superior performance in about half the participants. To test this, we used the binomial cumulative


distribution function (binocdf, MATLAB, MathWorks) to assess the probability of our data (observing superior performance on intervention night relative to undisturbed night for 6/6


participants), against the null hypothesis that the probability for each participant to have superior performance on stimulation nights is 0.5. To obtain estimates and 95% confidence


intervals of the percentage of participants who had superior performance on intervention nights, we computed the likelihood of the data given a binomial model (binofit, MATLAB, MathWorks).


To estimate confidence intervals for individual participants’ intervention efficacy, we ran a bootstrapping procedure as follows: for every test set, we selected 25 images and 15 lures (with


repetitions) out of each test’s image set and calculated participant scores based on the selected set. We repeated this 1,000 times for each participant. We calculated the mean and standard


deviation of the bootstrapped scores for correct recognitions, false alarms and recognition memory accuracy for each participant (plotted in Extended Data Fig. 4c–e). We also computed the


mean across participants for each bootstrap iteration to estimate a distribution of means (shown in the insets of Extended Data Fig. 4c–e). Before the evening learning session and following


the morning testing session, participants performed a face/non-face categorization PVT as described in ref. 38 to quantify vigilance. In brief, during each block, four face images and two


non-face images (places or animals) were presented on a laptop computer for 200 ms while participants performed a face/non-face categorization task. Each picture was presented 24 times in a


pseudorandom order (total of 144 trials), with long pseudorandomized inter-stimulus intervals of 2–8 s (uniform distribution), as in classical PVT designs80. Participants were instructed to


press one of two buttons (for face versus non-face) as quickly as possible. We used the difference between median reaction times between morning and evening in the visual PVT task to assess


changes in vigilance following intervention nights and undisturbed nights (Extended Data Fig. 4f,g). Subjective sleepiness was assessed at the beginning of each experimental session using


the Stanford Sleepiness Scale and a visual analog rating of sleepiness. ELECTRODE LOCALIZATION Depth electrode placement was determined solely based on clinical considerations. Before


electrode implantation, we obtained for each participant a T1-weighted 1-mm isometric structural MR scan using a 3-Tesla scanner. After implantation, a CT scan was acquired and co-registered


via an affine transform to the preoperative anatomical MR scan (after skull stripping) using FSL’s BET and FLIRT toolbox (FMRIB v6.0)81,82,83. This allowed visualization of the CT scan


superimposed with the preoperative MRI scan (Fig. 1c and Extended Data Fig. 2). Individual recording sites were then identified visually on the co-registered CT and manually marked in each


participant’s preoperative MRI native space using BioImage Suite (RRID: SCR_016109)84. Co-registration and electrode localization were performed using the iELVIS toolbox84: The


preimplantation three-dimensional T1 MR scan was processed using FreeSurfer to segment the white matter, deep gray matter structures and cortex; and to parcellate the neocortex according to


gyral anatomy85,86. Each iEEG electrode was then attributed to a cortical region according to automated parcellation in FreeSurfer85. We warped the aligned electrodes onto a standard brain


template (using MNI template) to facilitate group-level visualization (Figs. 1d, 2d, 3c and 4b). The MNI reconstruction was performed for visualization purposes only, and electrode


localizations were always determined in native MR space. All stimulation sites were verified to reside in white matter and all MTL probe locations in gray matter by a neurologist and a


neurosurgeon. iEEG electrode contacts used for ripple detection (see below) were verified to be in hippocampus/entorhinal cortex/parahippocampal gyrus gray matter. We defined MTL–neocortical


iEEG couples in the following manner (Fig. 4b): the ‘ripple channel’ was an MTL iEEG electrode with ripple detections in pre-stimulation sleep, while the ‘prefrontal channel’ was a


prefrontal iEEG electrode with the highest number of spindle event detections during pre-stimulation sleep in the same hemisphere (in one pair/participant, we paired MTL and prefrontal


contacts from opposite hemispheres). When using the MNI brain template to demonstrate widespread effects (Fig. 2d), score values outside the 5–95 percentiles are displayed in minimum/maximum


colors for better visualization. STIMULATION A board-certified neurologist was present in each stimulation session to monitor the clinical iEEG recordings for after-discharges and ensure


participant safety. Stimulation of epileptogenic areas was avoided when possible and the neurologist validated stimulation site and impedance before each session. Before every experimental


session, each participant was given a short series of test stimulation pulses while a neurologist monitored the clinical iEEG recordings for after-discharges and ensuring stimulations were


correctly delivered. Unaware of the exact timing of stimulation onset, participants were asked at the end of each session (in the morning) to report any unusual feelings or sensations.


Participants did not report any effects of stimulation, nor could they indicate when stimulation occurred during the night. In 12/19 sessions, experiments occurred >10 h since last


seizure, and in 7 sessions, experiments occurred >2 h since last seizure. A board-certified neurologist validated that no seizures were detected during intervention nights. Stimulation


was current regulated and charge balanced, with pulses set below the threshold for after discharge, which was identified based on pretesting (range: 1.0–2.0 mA). Stimulation electrode


impedance was measured immediately before testing (range, 1–4 kΩ, using clinical Neurofax EEG-1200A system, Nihon Koden). Stimulations were delivered in one of two schemes (Supplementary


Table 3): (i) bipolar stimulation (participants 1–3) used a CereStim R96 Macro-stimulator (BlackRock Microsystems) to deliver electrical stimulation to the Behnke–Fried depth electrode


bipolar macro-contacts spaced 3.5 mm apart (surface area, 0.06 cm2)69, and (ii) unipolar stimulation (participants 4–18) with the electrodes referenced to the EEG GND electrode. Each


50-ms-long stimulation event included five rectangular pulses (pulse width of 100 μs) at a frequency of 100 Hz, with the current ranging from 0.5 to 1.5 mA. Stimulation ranged between 2.5


and 7.6 μC of charge per square centimeter per phase, which is well below the safe maximum used for long-term and short-term stimulation (30 and 57 μC, respectively)87,88. TIMING OF


REAL-TIME CLOSED-LOOP STIMULATION BY ONLINE DETECTION OF SLOW WAVES IN MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE PROBE A neural signal processor (NSP; Cerebrus system, BlackRock Microsystems) connected to a


separate laptop was used to detect slow waves online to time electrical stimulation events. With this setup, the predefined probe’s iEEG signal was fed to a custom code running on the laptop


(MATLAB, MathWorks) in parallel to its recording by the NSP. The signal was low-pass filtered at <500 Hz and sampled at 2,000 Hz by the NSP. A custom-made script (running on MATLAB,


MathWorks) using the NSP’s application programming interface (BlackRock Microsystems) enabled responding to the incoming iEEG data in real time. To this end, the iEEG signal was further


band-passed filtered between 0.5 Hz and 4 Hz (50th order FIR filter). Thus, during 5-min stimulation blocks, each time the low-passed signal crossed an adaptive threshold toward larger


positive values, the electrical stimulation (details above) was triggered with a delay (details below). By default, the threshold was set to 80 µV, and the delay was either based on


pre-recorded sleep iEEG from participants or set to be 400 ms. The threshold was updated every 400 s to be the median iEEG amplitude of slow waves detected within the preceding a 400-s


interval, and the delay from detected peak to putative active phase was updated based on average values of peak to trough of detected slow events in the same interval. This algorithm ensured


a reliable way to continuously detect slow waves with dynamic amplitudes (for example, modulated by sleep depth and other factors) by their positive half-wave peaks40. The detection routine


was resumed 3 s after the delivery of stimulation to ensure sufficient buffers to evaluate slow-wave activity between stimulation events. After recording 7–15 min of uninterrupted NREM


sleep, stimulation blocks were administered in 5-min blocks, interleaved with 5-min pause blocks (‘pause’ intervals; Fig. 1b). At least 15–20 min of post-stimulation uninterrupted sleep data


were recorded after the last stimulation block. In a post hoc analysis, we measured the delay between each stimulation to the MTL probe’s peak that immediately preceded it (corresponding to


the down/inactive phase in iEEG signals32) and found the mean delay to be 241.3 ms for the sync-stimulation group and 373.3 ms for the mixed-stimulation group. We used post hoc analysis to


quantify the degree to which stimulation timings were in phase with MTL active periods: We required >55% of stimulations to be within 80–280 ms of the closest peak to be included in the


sync-stimulation group (Extended Data Fig. 3b). For participant 17 where post hoc analysis was not possible due to noise issues, we used the median delay from the peak based on the real-time


log. SLEEP SCORING Our approach focused on detecting epochs of NREM sleep based on iEEG signals. We were able to reliably detect NREM epochs in every recording, even when full


polysomnography (which would allow reliable separation of wakefulness from REM sleep) was not available. To guide the initiation of stimulation blocks online, visual detection of NREM sleep


epochs was performed at the participant bedside by a physician board certified in clinical neurophysiology. Visual detection focused on unequivocal presence of slow waves and sleep spindles


in iEEG data. To guide offline detailed analysis, automatic detection of NREM sleep was performed focusing on the presence of robust slow waves and sleep spindle activities in iEEG data89 as


follows. First, for each participant separately, we selected a neocortical iEEG channel distant from the seizure onset zone, where interictal activity was minimal, and—whenever


possible—from a contact placed in medial prefrontal/parietal cortex with prominent sleep spindle occurrence35. Second, after removing residual interictal discharge activities (see ‘iEEG


preprocessing and detection of pathological events’), we calculated the short-time Fourier transform (30-s window, no overlap, 0–40 Hz range, 0.2-Hz resolution; Fig. 1b). Note that an


additional normalization by a two-dimensional Gaussian filter (σ = 3) was used for visualization purposes in the figure but not for scoring. Third, we averaged the power in the slow-wave


(0.5–4 Hz) and spindle (9–16 Hz) frequency bands for each 30-s time point, resulting in two vectors representing slow-wave and spindle power. Fourth, we fit a two-component Gaussian mixture


distribution to the slow-wave–spindle joint vectors to represent synchronized NREM epochs versus desynchronized (REM/wakefulness) epochs. Fifth, we calculated the posterior probability for


each component given each time point (fitgmdist and posterior, MATLAB, MathWorks) and tagged each time point according to the maximal posterior probability (NREM versus desynchronized;


Extended Data Fig. 1a). Finally, standalone NREM detections (30 s only) that were >1 min away from other NREM detections were discarded according to AASM guidelines90. We validated our


approach by comparing the automated iEEG-based scheme used here with gold-standard polysomnography-based sleep scoring performed by an expert using AASM guidelines with three pre-scored


overnight recordings from an independent dataset32. The output of the data-driven Gaussian mixture approach was highly concordant with manual scoring (comparing the first 3 h of sleep in the


three different overnight datasets, we found a <4% change in the lengths of NREM sleep bouts). The approach used here was more robust to inter-participant variability compared with other


automated versions based only on predefined thresholds of delta power89. Importantly, post hoc analysis based on the iEEG-based sleep scoring confirmed that 76% ± 5.1% (average and s.e.m.


over _n_ = 19 sessions) of stimulation events occurred during NREM sleep. ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY DATA ACQUISITION AND OFFLINE SPIKE SORTING In each participant, 8–14 depth electrodes were


implanted targeting medial brain areas. Each depth electrode had eight platinum iEEG contacts along the shaft (Extended Data Fig. 6a(i), referenced to the scalp). Both scalp and depth iEEG


data were continuously recorded at a sampling rate of 2 kHz, band-pass filtered between 0.1 and 500 Hz, using either Blackrock or Neuralynx data acquisition systems. Each electrode


terminated in eight 40-mm platinum-iridium microwires from which extracellular signals were continuously recorded (Extended Data Fig. 6a; referenced locally to a ninth non-insulated


microwire) at a sampling rate of 28 or 30 kHz and band-pass-filtered between 1 and 6,000 Hz. SPIKE SORTING Neuronal clusters were identified using the ‘Wave Clus v2’ software package91 as


described previously32: Action potentials were detected by high-pass filtering the extracellular recordings above 300 Hz and applying a threshold at 5 s.d. above the median noise level.


Detected events were clustered (or categorized as noise) using automatic superparamagnetic clustering of wavelet coefficients, followed by manual refinement based on the consistency of spike


waveforms and inter-spike interval distributions (see example in Extended Data Fig. 6a(iii)). Unit stability throughout stimulation sessions was confirmed by verifying that spike waveforms


and inter-spike interval distributions were consistent and distinct in the interval ranging from pre-stimulation through post-stimulation time points (1–2 h; Supplementary Table 2 and


Extended Data Fig. 6a(iv)). Of 386 neural clusters identified by ‘Wave Clus’ (8 patients), 325 clusters (84%) were verified to be stable throughout the session and were included in further


analysis. INTRACRANIAL ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY PREPROCESSING AND DETECTION OF PATHOLOGICAL EVENTS Data analysis was performed with MATLAB (MathWorks), using the FieldTrip92 and CircStat93


toolboxes as well as custom scripts. Preprocessing of the iEEG data began with line noise removal (2-Hz band-stop filters centered at 60 Hz and its harmonics) and followed by an automated


algorithm to identify pathological events and electrical artifacts, as follows. First, for all data intervals occurring during NREM sleep, each time point was converted into a _z_-score


based on the participant-specific and stage-specific mean and s.d. of absolute amplitude, gradient (the amplitude difference between two adjacent time points) and amplitude of the data after


applying a 250-Hz high-pass filter. Next, epileptiform interictal spikes were detected automatically in iEEG signals by identifying events whose envelope of the high-passed signal was


larger than a threshold of +5 s.d., or a conjunction of absolute amplitude and gradient both passing a threshold of +5 s.d. and whose duration was <70 ms35. Points that passed the


detection condition and occurred in close temporal proximity (<50 ms) were merged as one interictal spike. Subsequent analysis of iEEG data was performed after detecting pathological


events in each channel separately: we used a semi-manual process, identifying channels with gross deviations of kurtosis/amplitude/skewness relative to other channels on the same electrode


and used visual validation as well as independent clinical neurologist channel-tagging to remove channels with high rates of interictal activity (>5 events per minute) or with electrical


noise. In channels included in further analysis, 500 ms preceding and following any interictal spike detection were removed. SINGLE-EVENT DETECTION OF SLEEP OSCILLATIONS Slow waves, sleep


spindle and MTL ripple events were identified independently for each participant and channel, based on established detection algorithms35,44. SLOW WAVES Slow waves were detected as in ref.


44. First, artifact-free iEEG signals from the NREM sleep stage were filtered between 0.16 Hz and 1.25 Hz (two-pass FIR band-pass filter, order = three cycles of the low-frequency cutoff).


Second, all zero-crossings were determined in the filtered signal, and event duration was determined for slow-wave candidates (that is, events consisting of an inactive/‘OFF’ period


corresponding to iEEG peak, followed by an active/’ON’ period corresponding to iEEG trough32) as the time between two successive negative-to-positive zero-crossings. For events whose


duration was between 0.8 s and 2 s, event amplitudes were determined (peak-to-trough amplitude between two negative-to-positive zero-crossings). Events that also met the amplitude criteria


(≥75% percentile of candidate amplitudes, that is, the 25% of events with the largest amplitudes) were considered as slow waves. SLEEP SPINDLES Spindles were detected automatically via a


two-step process based on ref. 35. First, to minimize false detections, only channels with robust spindle activity in NREM sleep were chosen for further analysis. To this end, in each


individual channel, sigma (9–16 Hz) power in NREM sleep was compared with a fitted 1/fα model (both were estimated across multiple 10-s epochs) and channels with a difference that was


statistically significant at _P_ < 0.001 (unpaired _t_-test for maximal peak) were further considered. Second, putative spindle events were selected based on their power and duration:


iEEG signals were band-pass filtered between 9 Hz and 16 Hz using a zero-phase fourth-order Butterworth filter. The instantaneous amplitude was computed via the Hilbert transform and two


thresholds were defined based on this amplitude time course across artifact-free sleep epochs. A detection threshold was set at the mean + 3 s.d. and amplitudes exceeding this threshold were


considered potential spindles. A start/end threshold was set at the mean + 1 s.d., and events whose duration was between 0.5 s and 2 s were further considered. Detections within 1 s were


merged as single events. We verified the spectral specificity of each spindle by excluding any detection that coincided with control events that were above the mean + 5 s.d in the 20–30 Hz


range. For single-event co-occurrence analysis (see below), we used a subpopulation of fast spindles where detection required a minimum frequency > 11 Hz. For every detected spindle, the


peaks and troughs were detected as the maxima and minima of the filtered signal, and the maximal peak was designated as the time point that represented the respective spindle in time (for


example, for single-event co-occurrence analysis and population average). MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE RIPPLES We utilized bipolar referencing to minimize effects of volume conduction by


identifying, in each electrode shaft targeting the MTL separately, a contact residing in white matter to be used as a reference for single-ripple detection, using preoperative and


postoperative CT and MRI data. We then used an automated detection algorithm as in ref. 44. First, data were filtered between 80 Hz and 100 Hz (two-pass FIR band-pass filter, order = three


cycles of the low-frequency cutoff), and only artifact-free data from NREM sleep were used for event detection. Second, the root-mean-square (RMS) signal was calculated for the filtered


signal using a moving average of 20 ms, and the ripple amplitude criterion was defined as the 99% percentile of RMS values. Third, whenever the signal exceeded this threshold for a minimum


of 38 ms (encompassing ~3 cycles at 80 Hz) a putative ripple event was marked. In addition, to avoid sharp broadband events, only those putative ripple events representing a true oscillatory


pattern were considered for further analysis. Accordingly, we focused on events with at least three discrete peaks or three discrete troughs in the raw signal corresponding to the


above-threshold RMS segment. This was accomplished by identifying local maxima or minima in the respective raw signal segments after applying a one-pass moving average filter including the


two adjacent data points. We demanded a detection of 20 ripples in pre-sleep baseline to include a contact as a ripple channel. Of 45 candidate MTL channels (18 participants), 17 were


excluded because ripple rate was too low (13) or baseline noise was too high (7). SINGLE-EVENT CO-OCCURRENCE Slow-wave–spindle sequences (Extended Data Fig. 5c,f) were defined similarly to


ref. 20 as epochs where spindle peaks occurred up to 1.5 s following iEEG slow-wave positive peak (down/OFF phase) on a specific iEEG contact. We also evaluated coupling incidence of single


sleep events in cross-brain electrode pairs. In each participant, we paired one contact from MTL electrodes with one contact from frontal-cortex electrode on the same hemisphere (when


possible, the superior temporal gyrus was used when the frontal electrode was not available, one participant only has a pair from opposite hemispheres), with maximal spindle activity. Of 55


candidate MTL–neocortical electrode pairs (18 participants), 13 were excluded because MTL channel was excluded from ripple analysis (see above). Of the resulting 42 couples, in one pair MTL


contact and neocortical contact were in different hemispheres, hence it was not included in the Fig. 4b visualization. MTL ripple–cortical slow-wave couples corresponded to epochs where


ripple peaks were 50–400 ms away from the slow-wave positive peak (down/OFF phase), including cortical contacts with >10 slow waves in the evaluated period. Co-occurrence of MTL ripple


and neocortical slow-wave/spindle sequences corresponded (similar to ref. 20) to an MTL ripple peak preceding a slow-wave–spindle sequence by 50–400 ms. STIMULATION-LOCKED TIME–FREQUENCY


ANALYSIS Stimulation-triggered analyses were performed for stimulation time points confirmed to occur during NREM sleep following post hoc sleep scoring (‘Sleep scoring’). TFRs (Figs. 2a and


4a) were extracted by calculating a spectrogram around stimulation events (0–2.5 s) and subtracting from it the pre-stimulation (−1 to 0 s) baseline spectrogram44. Spectrograms were


calculated using ft_specest_mtmconvol (FieldTrip toolbox92, MATLAB, MathWorks, frequencies 5–30 Hz, 1-Hz resolution) using a sliding Hanning-tapered window with a variable,


frequency-dependent, length that comprised at least five cycles44. Time-locked TFRs of all stimulation events were then normalized as the percentage change from pre-event baseline and were


averaged for each session (Fig. 2a). To estimate stimulation-locked average TFR increase in spindle frequency band, above and beyond the expected based on these time periods during slow-wave


active phases, we generated a set of sham-stimulation points as detailed and used those to calculate a sham-locked TFR as a baseline for comparison (Fig. 2(aii)). Slow-wave peaks were


detected in ‘pause’ (stimulation-free) 5-min blocks and a random subset of them was selected (equal in number to the number of real stimulations in the same session), sham events were then


selected with equal delay from peaks as the stimulations in the preceding block. Time-locked TFRs of all stimulation events and sham events were normalized as the percentage change from


pre-event baseline and averaged per session for each iEEG channel (Fig. 2(aii)). Sham points were selected offline using the same algorithm as used for online timing of stimulation events,


such that they also reflected MTL OFF–ON transitions, to control for the tendency of ON periods to be associated with greater spindle activity. We also performed this analysis with an


alternative selection of 1,000 random sham points during ‘pause’ sessions and observed similar results. SINGLE-EVENT PROBABILITY AND EVENT-RATE ESTIMATION To assess the probability of slow


waves, spindles, slow-wave–spindle couples and ripples following stimulations (Figs. 2 and 4), we detected single events (see above) on each iEEG contact separately. To evaluate the


immediate effect of stimulation on sleep oscillations (Fig. 2a and Extended Data Figs. 5a–c and 9a), we counted the detections during the 3 s following stimulation events (for slow waves and


spindles) and during 200 ms following stimulation events (for ripples). Probabilities were calculated as the sum of detections during immediate short periods following stimulation bursts,


divided by the number of stimulations in each session vectors. Note that for 3-s time vectors, event rates and probability values were very similar for slow waves and spindles, as they


typically do not occur more than once during these time periods35. As a within-session control, we used an equal number of sham time points (explained in the previous section, above) to


assess the degree to which stimulation increased event-detection probabilities, beyond the endogenous rates during active phases of slow waves. iEEG contacts with no detections in one of the


terms were excluded from the analysis. We normalized this change by contrasting the probability (P) to detect an event following stimulation with the probability calculated for sham time


points: \(\frac{{P_{\mathrm{stim}} - P_{\mathrm{sham}}}}{{P_{\mathrm{stim}} + P_{\mathrm{sham}}}}\). Spindle enhancement score per participant (Fig. 2d) was defined as the median of all


spindle-increase scores for all iEEG contacts, excluding contacts that did not have any spindle detections in either one of the conditions (stim/sham). For longer time epochs (the prolonged


condition in Figs. 2 and 4 and Extended Data Figs. 5, 8 and 9), event rates were calculated as the number of detected events divided by the accumulated length of time. Event enhancement


score in the prolonged condition (Fig. 2d) was similarly calculated as a contrast index between post-stimulation time vectors (1 min post-stimulation block) and the furthest equal-length


time period in the remaining ‘pause’ block (that is, 1 min before the following stimulation block): \(\frac{\mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} -


\mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{pre} - \mathrm{stim}}}{{\mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} + \mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{pre} -


\mathrm{stim}}}}\). ESTIMATING PHASE LOCKING OF NEURAL SPIKING ACTIVITY BY FITTING A COSINE FUNCTION All analyses were performed using MATLAB (MathWorks). To quantify the degree of phase


locking between MTL slow-wave oscillations and neuronal spiking activity in different brain regions (Fig. 3), we fitted a cosine function to the distribution of spike phases relative to


phase values of the MTL probe’s iEEG signal94. For each neural cluster and each condition (baseline, stimulation as described below), we repeated the fitting procedure to create a unique


lock-depth measure for each condition (Extended Data Fig. 6b). First, we computed the instantaneous amplitude of the MTL probe’s iEEG signal via the Hilbert transform following a Butterworth


band-pass filter between 0.5 Hz and 2 Hz (zero-phase filtering via filtfilt). We then extracted the probe’s phase for each neural spike. We used 20-degree bins to create a histogram of


spike phases (−180 < _φ_ < 180). We fitted every spike-phase histogram with the follomatlabwing function: \(f(\varphi ) = a \times \cos (\varphi + b) + c\) (Extended Data Fig. 6b). We


computed the _R_2 value between the original spike-histogram and the fit. We found _R_2 > 0.25 to have a good correspondence with the Rayleigh test for non-uniformity of phases


(calculated by circ_rtest), as >90% of distributions over this value had passed _P_ < 0.05, but the fitting procedure was less sensitive to variations in distribution shape than the


circular statistics. We defined an index to capture the phase-locking depth (LD) of oscillatory modulation in our fit as follows: \(\mathrm{LD} = \frac{{2a}}{{(2 \times a + c)}}\). We


included in our analysis 325 neural units validated as having a stable inter-spike-interval probability distribution (see above). For each condition, we analyzed phase locking for neural


units with a minimum mean firing rate of 0.1 Hz. We included in the phase-locking change analysis (Fig. 3c and Extended Data Fig. 6c) neural units with significant phase locking during


evaluated conditions (Rayleigh test, _P_ < 0.05). To describe in full the immediate and prolonged changes in phase locking, we defined several conditions for the evaluation of spiking


activity. To assess the change during stimulation blocks, baseline values were based on neural activity during the uninterrupted 5-min sleep period before the first intervention (‘PRE’; Fig.


1b). During stimulation blocks (used in Fig. 3b), we excluded spikes that occurred within the 500 ms following stimulation events. We defined a prolonged condition as all 1-min


post-stimulation blocks. Phase-locking depth change was evaluated by the following index (Fig. 3c,d and Extended Data Fig. 6c, d): \(\frac{{\mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} -


\mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{baseline}}}}{{\mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} + \mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{baseline}}}}\). We compared locking depth for units that had significant phase locking


in compared conditions (Rayleigh test, _P_ < 0.05). Note that the number of clusters with significant phase locking increased during the stimulation session, as demonstrated in Fig. 3b.


These changes resulted in a varying number of units for each evaluated condition. We calculated lock-depth change for all various combinations of evaluated conditions versus the two


different baselines used in the main text (‘PRE’ sleep and 1-min pre-stimulation blocks), all in agreement with the main analysis reported in Fig. 3c,d (see full distributions and statistics


in Extended Data Fig. 6c). We performed two separate shuffling procedures to validate the prolonged effect in phase locking—comparing the prolonged condition to the 1-min period before


stimulation blocks (Fig. 3d): (1) We assigned lock-depth values to the tested condition or baseline randomly (randperm, MATLAB, MathWorks) 10,000 times and tested whether the randomly


shuffled distribution differed significantly from the calculated distribution (Extended Data Fig. 6c). (2) To test against the hypothesis that changes in firing rates bias our phase-locking


calculation, we performed the following procedure 1,000 times—for each comparison between conditions, we selected a set of _X_ spikes from each condition; _X_ totaled 90% of the minimum


spike count of the two conditions. We refit a cosine as described above for both selected sets and recalculated phase-locking change. The resulting distribution was not significantly


different from the distribution calculated based on the full number of spikes and significantly passed the Wilcoxon sign-rank test as the distribution reported in Fig. 3d (Extended Data Fig.


6c). STATISTICAL ANALYSES We used parametric methods for statistical testing of normal data. For non-normal data or small sample sizes, we used Wilcoxon signed-rank/rank-sum tests. To


compare two distributions, we used the Kolmogorov–Smirnov two-sample test. All statistical tests were two sided unless stated otherwise. In violin plots representing estimated distributions


of data (generated with violinplot, FieldTrip toolbox92, MATLAB, MathWorks), lines represent 5, 50 and 95 percentiles. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample sizes but our


sample sizes are similar to those generally used in previous publications44,73,95. Data collection and analysis were not performed blind to the conditions of the experiments. REPORTING


SUMMARY Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article. DATA AVAILABILITY Supplementary tables supporting the findings


of this paper are available as Supplementary Information. Source data are provided with this paper. CODE AVAILABILITY Toolboxes developed by authors for the detection of single events in


iEEG data are freely available on GitHub: https://github.com/mgevasagiv/sleepScoringIEEG, sleep scoring based on iEEG data; https://github.com/mgevasagiv/rippleDetection_IEEG, ripple


detection in iEEG data; https://github.com/mgevasagiv/epilepticActivity_IEEG, IED detection in iEEG data; https://github.com/mgevasagiv/sleepOscillations_IEEG, slow-wave and spindle


detection in iEEG data. Additional code used for data analysis is available on reasonable request from the corresponding authors. REFERENCES * Jenkins, J. G. & Dallenbach, K. M.


Obliviscence during sleep and waking. _Am. J. Psychol._ 35, 605–612 (1924). Article  Google Scholar  * Maquet, P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. _Science_ 294, 1048–1052 (2001).


Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Walker, M. P. & Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. _Neuron_ 44, 121–133 (2004). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar


  * Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. The memory function of sleep. _Nat. Rev. Neurosci._ 11, 114–126 (2010). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Squire, L. R. & Zola, S. M. Structure


and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 93, 13515–13522 (1996). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Marr, D. Simple


memory: a theory for archicortex. _Philos. Trans. R. Soc. L. B Biol. Sci._ 262, 23–81 (1971). CAS  Google Scholar  * Buzsáki, G. Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for ‘noisy’


brain states. _Neuroscience_ 31, 551–570 (1989). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Buzsáki, G. The hippocampo–neocortical dialogue. _Cereb. Cortex_ 6, 81–92 (1996). Article  PubMed  Google


Scholar  * Vaz, A. P., Inati, S. K., Brunel, N. & Zaghloul, K. A. Coupled ripple oscillations between the medial temporal lobe and neocortex retrieve human memory. _Science_ 363,


975–978 (2019). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Liu, A. A. et al. A consensus statement on detection of hippocampal sharp wave ripples and differentiation from other


fast oscillations. _Nat. Commun._ 13, 6000 (2022). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Girardeau, G. & Lopes-Dos-Santos, V. Brain neural patterns and the memory


function of sleep. _Science_ 374, 560–564 (2021). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Logothetis, N. K. et al. Hippocampal–cortical interaction during periods of


subcortical silence. _Nature_ 491, 547–553 (2012). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Skelin, I. et al. Coupling between slow waves and sharp-wave ripples engages distributed neural


activity during sleep in humans. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 118, e2012075118 (2021). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Steriade, M. Grouping of brain rhythms in


corticothalamic systems. _Neuroscience_ 137, 1087–1106 (2006). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Timofeev, I. Neuronal plasticity and thalamocortical sleep and waking oscillations.


_Prog. Brain Res._ 193, 121–144 (2011). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Klinzing, J. G., Niethard, N. & Born, J. Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during


sleep. _Nat. Neurosci._ 22, 1598–1610 (2019). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Ohki, T. & Takei, Y. Neural mechanisms of mental schema: a triplet of delta, low beta/spindle and


ripple oscillations. _Eur. J. Neurosci._ 48, 2416–2430 (2018). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Lewis, P. A., Cairney, S., Manning, L. & Critchley, H. D. The impact of overnight


consolidation upon memory for emotional and neutral encoding contexts. _Neuropsychologia_ 49, 2619–2629 (2011). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Talamini, L. M.,


Nieuwenhuis, I. L. C., Takashima, A. & Jensen, O. Sleep directly following learning benefits consolidation of spatial associative memory. _Learn. Mem._ 15, 233–237 (2008). Article 


PubMed  Google Scholar  * Maingret, N., Girardeau, G., Todorova, R., Goutierre, M. & Zugaro, M. Hippocampo–cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep. _Nat. Neurosci._


19, 959–964 (2016). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Latchoumane, C. V., Ngo, H. V., Born, J. & Shin, H. S. Thalamic spindles promote memory formation during sleep through


triple-phase locking of cortical, thalamic, and hippocampal rhythms. _Neuron_ 95, 424–435 (2017). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Siapas, A. G. & Wilson, M. A. Coordinated


interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. _Neuron_ 21, 1123–1128 (1998). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Binder, S. et al. Monosynaptic


hippocampal–prefrontal projections contribute to spatial memory consolidation in mice. _J. Neurosci._ 39, 6978–6991 (2019). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Benthem,


S. D. et al. Impaired hippocampal–cortical interactions during sleep in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. _Curr. Biol._ 30, 2588–2601 (2020). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google


Scholar  * Girardeau, G., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I., Buzsáki, G. & Zugaro, M. B. Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. _Nat. Neurosci._ 12, 1222–1223


(2009). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Ego‐Stengel, V. & Wilson, M. A. Disruption of ripple‐associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat.


_Hippocampus_ 20, 1–10 (2010). PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Fernandez-Ruiz, A. et al. Long-duration hippocampal sharp wave ripples improve memory. _Science_ 364, 1082–1086


(2019). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Helfrich, R. F. et al. Bidirectional prefrontal–hippocampal dynamics organize information transfer during sleep in humans.


_Nat. Commun._ 10, 3572 (2019). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Sanda, P. et al. Bidirectional interaction of hippocampal ripples and cortical slow waves leads to


coordinated spiking activity during NREM sleep. _Cereb. Cortex_ 31, 324–340 (2021). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Yonelinas, A. P., Ranganath, C., Ekstrom, A. D. & Wiltgen, B. J. A


contextual binding theory of episodic memory: systems consolidation reconsidered. _Nat. Rev. Neurosci._ 20, 364–375 (2019). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Hoppe, C.,


Elger, C. E. & Helmstaedter, C. Long-term memory impairment in patients with focal epilepsy. _Epilepsia_ 48, 26–29 (2007). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Nir, Y. et al. Regional


slow waves and spindles in human sleep. _Neuron_ 70, 153–169 (2011). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Sirota, A., Csicsvari, J., Buhl, D. & Buzsáki, G.


Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 100, 2065–2069 (2003). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  *


Eichenbaum, H. Prefrontal–hippocampal interactions in episodic memory. _Nat. Rev. Neurosci._ 18, 547–558 (2017). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Andrillon, T. et al. Sleep spindles


in humans: insights from intracranial EEG and unit recordings. _J. Neurosci._ 31, 17821–17834 (2011). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Geva-Sagiv, M. & Nir, Y.


Local sleep oscillations: implications for memory consolidation. _Front. Neurosci._ 13, 813 (2019). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Mankin, E. A. & Fried, I.


Modulation of human memory by deep brain stimulation of the entorhinal–hippocampal circuitry. _Neuron_ 106, 218–235 (2020). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Nir, Y. et


al. Selective neuronal lapses precede human cognitive lapses upon sleep deprivation. _Nat. Med._ 23, 1474–1480 (2017). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Gais, S.,


Mölle, M., Helms, K. & Born, J. Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. _J. Neurosci._ 22, 6830–6834 (2002). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Ngo,


H. V., Martinetz, T., Born, J. & Mölle, M. Auditory closed-loop stimulation of the sleep slow oscillation enhances memory. _Neuron_ 78, 545–553 (2013). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google


Scholar  * Bar, E. et al. Local targeted memory reactivation in human sleep. _Curr. Biol._ 30, 1435–1446 (2020). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Rasch, B. & Born, J. About


sleep’s role in memory. _Physiol. Rev._ 93, 681–766 (2013). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Steriade, M., Nunez, A. & Amzica, F. Intracellular analysis of


relations between the slow (<1 Hz) neocortical oscillation and other sleep rhythms of the electroencephalogram. _J. Neurosci._ 13, 3266–3283 (1993). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central 


Google Scholar  * Staresina, B. P. et al. Hierarchical nesting of slow oscillations, spindles and ripples in the human hippocampus during sleep. _Nat. Neurosci._ 18, 1679–1686 (2015).


Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Bragin, A., Engel, J. Jr., Wilson, C. L., Fried, I. & Buzsáki, G. High-frequency oscillations in human brain. _Hippocampus_ 9,


137–142 (1999). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Clemens, Z. et al. Temporal coupling of parahippocampal ripples, sleep spindles and slow oscillations in humans. _Brain_ 130,


2868–2878 (2007). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Sakon, J. J. & Kahana, M. J. Hippocampal ripples signal contextually mediated episodic recall. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 119,


e2201657119 (2022). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Mölle, M., Bergmann, T. O., Marshall, L. & Born, J. Fast and slow spindles during the sleep slow oscillation:


disparate coalescence and engagement in memory processing. _Sleep_ 34, 1411–1421 (2011). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Schabus, M. et al. Hemodynamic cerebral correlates


of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 104, 13164–13169 (2007). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Vyazovskiy, V. V.,


Faraguna, U., Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Triggering slow waves during NREM sleep in the rat by intracortical electrical stimulation: effects of sleep/wake history and background activity.


_J. Neurophysiol._ 101, 1921–1931 (2009). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Suthana, N. et al. Memory enhancement and deep brain stimulation of the entorhinal area. _N.


Engl. J. Med._ 366, 502–510 (2012). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Rajasethupathy, P., Ferenczi, E. & Deisseroth, K. Targeting neural circuits. _Cell_ 165,


524–534 (2016). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Mohan, U. R. et al. The effects of direct brain stimulation in humans depend on frequency, amplitude, and white-matter


proximity. _Brain Stimul._ 13, 1183–1195 (2020). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Mayberg, H. S. et al. Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. _Neuron_


45, 651–660 (2005). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Schmidig, F. et al. A visual paired associate learning (vPAL) paradigm to study memory consolidation during sleep. Preprint at


_bioRxiv_ https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.03.28.534494 (2023). * Jacoby, L. L., Shimizu, Y., Daniels, K. A. & Rhodes, M. G. Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory:


depth of retrieval. _Psychon. Bull. Rev._ 12, 852–857 (2005). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Payne, J. D. et al. The role of sleep in false memory formation. _Neurobiol. Learn. Mem._ 92,


327–334 (2009). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Diekelmann, S., Born, J. & Wagner, U. Sleep enhances false memories depending on general memory performance. _Behav.


Brain Res._ 208, 425–429 (2010). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Huan, S.-Y., Xu, H.-Z., Wang, R. & Yu, J. The different roles of sleep on false memory formation between young and


older adults. _Psychol. Res._ 86, 443–451 (2022). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Nir, Y., Le Van Quyen, M., Tononi, G. & Staba, R. J. Microelectrode studies of human sleep. in


_Single Neuron Studies of the Human Brain: Probing Cognition_ (eds. I. Fried et al.) 65–188 (The MIT Press, 2014). * Crespel, A., Coubes, P. & Baldy-Moulinier, M. Sleep influence on


seizures and epilepsy effects on sleep in partial frontal and temporal lobe epilepsies. _Clin. Neurophysiol._ 111, S54–S59 (2000). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Bazil, C. W. Effects of


antiepileptic drugs on sleep structure: are all drugs equal? _CNS Drugs_ 17, 719–728 (2003). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Boly, M. et al. Altered sleep homeostasis correlates with


cognitive impairment in patients with focal epilepsy. _Brain_ 140, 1026–1040 (2017). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Klimes, P. et al. NREM sleep is the state of


vigilance that best identifies the epileptogenic zone in the interictal electroencephalogram. _Epilepsia_ 60, 2404–2415 (2019). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Steriade, M., Contreras, D.


& Amzica, F. Synchronized sleep oscillations and their paroxysmal developments. _Trends Neurosci._ 17, 199–208 (1994). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Gelinas, J. N.,


Khodagholy, D., Thesen, T., Devinsky, O. & Buzsáki, G. Interictal epileptiform discharges induce hippocampal-cortical coupling in temporal lobe epilepsy. _Nat. Med._ 22, 641–648 (2016).


Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Ezzyat, Y. et al. Closed-loop stimulation of temporal cortex rescues functional networks and improves memory. _Nat. Commun._ 9, 365


(2018). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Titiz, A. S. et al. Theta-burst microstimulation in the human entorhinal area improves memory specificity. _Elife_ 6, e29515


(2016). Article  Google Scholar  * Mankin, E. A. et al. Stimulation of the right entorhinal white matter enhances visual memory encoding in humans. _Brain Stimul._ 14, 131–140 (2021).


Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Marshall, L. & Born, J. The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. _Trends Cogn. Sci._ 11, 442–450 (2007). Article 


PubMed  Google Scholar  * Ong, J. L. et al. Effects of phase-locked acoustic stimulation during a nap on EEG spectra and declarative memory consolidation. _Sleep. Med._ 20, 88–97 (2016).


Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Leminen, M. M. et al. Enhanced memory consolidation via automatic sound stimulation during non-REM sleep. _Sleep_ 40, zsx003 (2017). Article  PubMed 


PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Henin, S. et al. Closed-loop acoustic stimulation enhances sleep oscillations but not memory performance. _eNeuro_ 6, ENEURO.0306-19.2019 (2019). * Cordi,


M. J. & Rasch, B. How robust are sleep-mediated memory benefits? _Curr. Opin. Neurobiol._ 67, 1–7 (2021). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Batterink, L. J., Creery, J. D. &


Paller, K. A. Phase of spontaneous slow oscillations during sleep influences memory-related processing of auditory cues. _J. Neurosci._ 36, 1401–1409 (2016). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed


Central  Google Scholar  * Fried, I. et al. Cerebral microdialysis combined with single-neuron and electroencephalographic recording in neurosurgical patients. _J. Neurosurg._ 91, 697–705


(1999). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Marshall, L., Mölle, M., Hallschmid, M. & Born, J. Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. _J.


Neurosci._ 24, 9985–9992 (2004). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Plihal, W. & Born, J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural


memory. _J. Cogn. Neurosci._ 9, 534–547 (1997). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Tadros, T. & Bazhenov, M. Role of sleep in formation of relational associative memory. _J.


Neurosci._ 42, 5330–5345 (2022). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Lim, J. & Dinges, D. F. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. _Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci._ 1129,


305–322 (2008). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Jenkinson, M. & Smith, S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. _Med. Image Anal._ 5, 143–156


(2001). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M. & Smith, S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion


correction of brain images. _Neuroimage_ 17, 825–841 (2002). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Smith, S. M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. _Hum. Brain Mapp._ 17, 143–155 (2002).


Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Groppe, D. M. et al. iELVis: An open source MATLAB toolbox for localizing and visualizing human intracranial electrode data. _J. Neurosci.


Methods_ 281, 40–48 (2017). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Desikan, R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based


regions of interest. _Neuroimage_ 31, 968–980 (2006). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Fischl, B. FreeSurfer. _Neuroimage_ 62, 774–781 (2012). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Gordon,


B. et al. Parameters for direct cortical electrical stimulation in the human: histopathologic confirmation. _Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol._ 75, 371–377 (1990). Article  CAS  PubMed


  Google Scholar  * Agnew, W. F. & McCreery, D. B. Considerations for safety with chronically implanted nerve electrodes. _Epilepsia_ 31, S27–S32 (1990). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar


  * Ramot, M. et al. Emergence of sensory patterns during sleep highlights differential dynamics of REM and non-REM sleep stages. _J. Neurosci._ 33, 14715–14728 (2013). Article  CAS  PubMed


  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A. L. & Quan, S. F. _AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associate events. Rules, terminology and technical


specifications_ (American Association of Sleep Medicine, 2007). * Quiroga, R. Q., Nadasdy, Z. & Ben-Shaul, Y. Unsupervised spike detection and sorting with wavelets and


superparamagnetic clustering. _Neural Comput._ 16, 1661–1687 (2004). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E. & Schoffelen, J.-M. FieldTrip: open source


software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. _Comput. Intell. Neurosci._ 2011, 156869 (2011). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Berens, P. CircStat: a


MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. _J. Stat. Softw._ 31, 1–21 (2009). Article  Google Scholar  * Eliav, T. et al. Nonoscillatory phase coding and synchronization in the bat hippocampal


formation. _Cell_ 175, 1119–1130 (2018). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Lafon, B. et al. Low frequency transcranial electrical stimulation does not entrain sleep rhythms measured


by human intracranial recordings. _Nat. Commun._ 8, 1199 (2017). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank the participants in this


study. We thank I.F., Y.N. and C. Ranganath lab members for discussions and comments on the manuscript. We also thank N. Ulanovsky, C. Ranganath, Z. Aghajan, N. Suthana, R. Staba and F.


Fahoum for their valuable advice throughout the project; B. Salaz and N. Regev for administrative assistance; S. Benard, V. Shah, C. Elder, R. Mazumder and V. Ho for medical oversight; M.


Tran for assistance with neural recordings; K. Shattuck and T. Fields for technical assistance; Y. Norman and H. Hayat for iELVIS training; A. Lin and S. Jalal for statistical consultation;


and S. Tzach for graphics. This study was supported by research grants from the National Science Foundation and US-Israel Binational Science Foundation (1756473, 2017628) to I.F. and Y.N.,


National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (U01 grants NS108930 and NS123128, R01-NS084017 to I.F.), European Research Council (ERC-2019-CoG 864353 to Y.N.). M.G.-S. was funded


by a Postdoctoral fellowship from the Human Frontier Science Program Organization (LT000440), a Postdoctoral fellowship from the Rothschild Foundation, a Tel Aviv University Sagol School of


Neuroscience Postdoctoral Fellowship, The Israel National Postdoctoral Program for Advancing Women in Science and a travel grant by Tel Aviv University GRTF and The Naomi Foundation travel


grants. E.A.M. was funded by a Postdoctoral Fellowship from the A.P. Giannini Foundation and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke grant NS058280. AUTHOR INFORMATION Author


notes * Maya Geva-Sagiv Present address: Center of Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA * These authors jointly supervised this work: Yuval Nir, Itzhak Fried.


AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Neurosurgery, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA Maya Geva-Sagiv, Emily A. Mankin, Natalie Cherry, Guldamla Kalender, 


Natalia Tchemodanov & Itzhak Fried * Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel Maya Geva-Sagiv & Yuval Nir * Department of Neurology, University of


California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA Dawn Eliashiv * Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Sackler School of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel Shdema Epstein 


& Yuval Nir * Department of Biomedical Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel Yuval Nir Authors * Maya Geva-Sagiv View author publications You can


also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Emily A. Mankin View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Dawn Eliashiv View author


publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Shdema Epstein View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Natalie


Cherry View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Guldamla Kalender View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google


Scholar * Natalia Tchemodanov View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Yuval Nir View author publications You can also search for this author


inPubMed Google Scholar * Itzhak Fried View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS M.G.-S., Y.N. and I.F. conceived research and


designed experiments. M.G.-S., E.A.M., Y.N. and I.F. secured funding. M.G.-S., E.A.M. and N.T. designed and assembled the RTCL system. M.G.-S., E.A.M., N.C. and G.K. collected data. M.G.-S.


analyzed data, supervised by Y.N. S.E contributed analysis tools. I.F. performed surgeries and supervised data collection and execution of experiments. D.E. supervised clinical care,


stimulation safety and analyzed epilepsy profiles. M.G.-S., I.F. and Y.N. wrote the manuscript. All authors provided ongoing critical review of results and commented on the manuscript.


CORRESPONDING AUTHORS Correspondence to Yuval Nir or Itzhak Fried. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS M.G.-S., Y.N. and I.F. are co-inventors of a US patent related to this


investigation. The authors declare no other competing interests. PEER REVIEW PEER REVIEW INFORMATION _Nature Neuroscience_ thanks Ueli Rutishauser, Igor Timofeev, and the other, anonymous,


reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER’S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published


maps and institutional affiliations. EXTENDED DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 1 AUTOMATED SCORING OF NREM SLEEP INTERVALS BASED ON IEEG. An example of overnight NREM detection performed on


orbitofrontal cortex iEEG activity, used for sleep scoring in participant #3 (full spectrogram: Fig. 1b). (A) Scatter plot of spindle power (9–16 Hz) versus slow-wave power (0.5–4 Hz). Each


dot marks a 30 sec epoch, and its color denotes scoring as NREM (red) or desynchronized (REM sleep/wakefulness, green), according to the maximum posterior probability of a 2-component


Gaussian mixture fit to the entire dataset. (B) iEEG power spectrum for each vigilance state for participant #3. Red: NREM sleep. Green: overnight desynchronized. Gray: unequivocal


wakefulness periods occurring before or after the overnight sleep session. Note that iEEG power spectrum during overnight desynchronized states (green) resembles that found for unequivocal


wakefulness (gray). (C) (i) Grand mean iEEG power spectra over all participants for all sleep/wake stages (n = 19 overnight sessions). Colors as in b. (ii-iv) Solid lines show the mean


spectra across all patients; Dashed lines denote power spectrum per patient for NREM sleep (ii), overnight desynchronized states(iii), and unequivocal wakefulness periods(iv). Source data


EXTENDED DATA FIG. 2 LOCATION OF MTL SYNCHRONIZATION-PROBE AND NEOCORTICAL STIMULATION IEEG ELECTRODES. For each participant (p1–18), two coronal MR images show the locations of the MTL


synchronization-probe for closed-loop control (left image, blue circles) and neocortical stimulation site (right image, red circles, for bipolar stimulations the adjacent contact was used as


reference). Yellow circles depict other iEEG contacts on the depth electrode. Title for each MR image: p = participant, number corresponds to participant-id in Supplementary Tables 1–4.


Then the location of the highlighted iEEG contact. R = right, L = Left; PF = prefrontal cortex, T = temporal cortex, TO = Temporal-occipital cortex, AH = Anterior Hippocampus, MH = Middle


hippocampus, EC = Entorhinal cortex, PHG = Parahippocampal gyrus. Note that participants 5–7,15 are shown at the bottom, as stimulation site was outside the prefrontal lobe. EXTENDED DATA


FIG. 3 CLOSED LOOP SYSTEM. (A) Example from participant #7 showing the average and SEM of MTL probe’s iEEG signal (blue trace, filtered between [0.5–4]Hz to highlight slow-wave activity),


time-locked to the positive iEEG peak immediately preceding stimulation time (t = 0). Note that iEEG peak corresponds to the neuronal inactive slow-wave phase. Top inset: distribution of


stimulation delays (n = 423 stimulation events during period highlighted by gray background) from iEEG slow-wave positive peak for this participant. (B) Our phase targeting method was based


on detecting peaks in the iEEG signal and delivering stimulation at a pre-determined delay following the peak. We quantified the degree to which stimulations were delivered in phase with MTL


active periods _post hoc_. All sync-stimulation patients (red, brown) had >60% stimulations delivered in the planned delay range, while mixed-phased patients (gray) had <45%


stimulations within that range. Two subpanels on the right depict two representative distributions of stimulation delays in two patients – patient #18 from sync-stim group (top) and patient


#1 from the mixed-phase group (bottom). (C) Individual immediate effect of spindle increase reveal significant positive correlation to the percentage of in-range stimulations (Spearman


correlation: ρ = 0.51, *P = 0.027, n = 18 nights). The distribution across all iEEG contacts for each stimulation night is shown; black crosses mark the mean spindle enhancement in each


subject. Red, sync-stimulation in prefrontal cortex. Brown, sync-stimulation in other neocortical regions. Gray, mixed-phase stimulation in prefrontal cortex. (D) Memory enhancement (as in


Fig. 1g) is positively correlated with the percentage of in-range stimulations (Methods, Spearman correlation: ρ = 0.40, P = 0.19, n = 12 patients). Source data EXTENDED DATA FIG. 4


BEHAVIORAL MEASURES. (A, B) ASSOCIATION TESTING: Participants were asked to recall the animal associated with every person they recognized from the learning session. Pairing index (PI;


100*number correct/number attempted). (A) Overnight change PIMorning-PIEvening is plotted for participants who were tested following undisturbed sleep (left) and following a sleep with RTCL


stimulation (right). Line color depicts stimulation type: red, synchronizing stimulation (n = 5, one participant chose not to complete the association test after undisturbed sleep night);


brown, synchronizing stimulation, delivered in other regions (n = 2); gray, mixed-phase stimulation in prefrontal neocortex (n = 3). (B) Within-subject difference of overnight change in


pairing success between intervention night and undisturbed sleep (difference between the dots in panel a): 5 of 7 participants with sync-stimulation (red, brown) showed either no change or


superior performance in stimulation nights (Stimulation – Sleep > = 0), while only 1 of 3 participants with mixed-phase stimulation (gray) showed this effect. There were no significant


changes in pairing success rates following RTCL stimulation relative to undisturbed nights. (C–E) Estimating effect size for recognition memory accuracy and its components (hit rate and


false alarm rate): we performed bootstrapping (n = 1000) by selecting a random sample (with replacement) from each night’s image set and recalculating each memory evaluation measure on the


subsampled data for each patient. The main panel displays the mean and standard deviation of the bootstrapped values for each patient, and the inset shows the distribution of the means


across patients for each bootstrapping cycle, aggregating all sync-stim patients in red and mixed-phase patients in gray. (C) Recognition memory accuracy. The mean change in memory accuracy


for the sync-stim averaged bootstrapped group trends toward improvement, but does not reach the 5% significance level, as the 95% confidence interval contains 0. (D) Correctly recognized


images (hits). Each intervention had minimal effect on this measure. (E) Wrongly identified lures (false alarms). None of the frontal-lobe sync-stimulation subjects (red) exhibited an


increase in the number of false alarms, while all mixed-phase subjects exhibited such an increase. The distribution of estimated means indicates a trend toward a decrease in false alarms for


the sync-stimulation subjects and a significant increase in false alarm rate for the mixed-phase stimulation subjects. This suggests that the difference in performance following the two


types of stimulation can be attributed to distinct effects on source memory. (F, G) Reaction time (RT) changes: (F) Change in mean RT for recognized images was not significantly different


between intervention nights (red) and undisturbed nights (green) (n = 9, P = 0.65; Wilcoxon rank-sum test). (G) RT change on a separate psychomotor vigilance task (PVT, see Methods) shows


significantly faster performance following undisturbed sleep than after sleep with RTCL stimulation (n = 11, P = 0.01; Wilcoxon rank-sum test). The bounds of the boxes (panels f, g)


represent the interquartile range and whiskers extend between 1–99 percentiles. Source data EXTENDED DATA FIG. 5 BRAIN-WIDE CHANGE IN SLEEP OSCILLATION RATES FOLLOWING SYNC-STIMULATION.


(A-C) Distributions of immediate change of detection probabilities for slow waves (A), spindles (B), and slow wave-spindle couples (C) for contacts across the brain. Probability was


calculated in 3 sec intervals immediately following stimulations, relative to SHAM-stimulation control points (as in Fig. 2c). These distributions reveal decreased probability of slow waves


in participants from both stimulation groups (red: sync stim; gray: mixed-phase stim). Wilcoxon signed rank tests are reported for each distribution and rank sum for comparing both


distributions: (A) slow-waves: P = 8*10−80 (sync), P = 3*10−32 (mixed), P = 0.08 (between groups), n = 556/215 iEEG contacts for red/gray groups. (B) Spindles: P = 6*10−8 (sync), P = 4*10−17


(mixed), P = 3*10−23 (between groups), n = 508/212 iEEG contacts for red/gray groups. (C) Slow wave-spindle couples: P = 1.9*10−5 (sync), P = 5*10−8 (mixed), P = 0.056 (between groups), n =


 333/169 iEEG contacts for red/gray groups. n-values differ between panels (a–c) because channels with zero detections in one of the conditions were excluded. Sub-panels depict the


distribution of baseline probabilities which are not significantly different between the two stimulation-mode groups. (D–I) Distributions of prolonged changes of detection rate for


slow-waves, spindles, and slow wave-spindle couples for channels outside the MTL (D–F) and MTL channels (G–I). Event rates were calculated over 1-min following stimulations-blocks (yellow


shade in top illustration), relative to an equal time range at the end of each ‘pause’ block (gray shade in top illustration). Panels display the difference between rates (events/min). These


distributions reveal an increase in spindle event rate in iEEG contacts in the sync-stimulation condition that decays during ‘pause’ blocks, while mixed-phase stimulation contacts exhibit


either no change or reduced rates immediately after stim blocks. Wilcoxon signed rank tests are reported for each distribution and rank sum for comparing both distributions: (D) slow-waves:


P = 0.4 (sync) P = 2*10−15 (mixed), P = 4.6*10−10 (between groups), n = 269/161 iEEG contacts for red/gray groups; (E) Spindles: P = 2*10−14 (sync) P = 0.017 (mixed), P = 1.2*10−12 (between


groups), n = 208/157 iEEG contacts for red/gray groups; (F) Slow-wave – spindle couples: P = 1.72*10−4 (sync), P = 0.3 (mixed), P = 0.003 (between groups), n = 135/110 iEEG contacts for


red/gray groups. N-values differ between panels (d-f) because channels with zero detection in one of the conditions were excluded. (G) slow-waves: P = 0.29 (sync) P = 0.053 (mixed), P = 


0.052 (between groups), n = 167/54 iEEG contacts for red/gray groups; (H) Spindles: P = 9*10−5 (sync) P = 0.27 (mixed), P = 0.086 (between groups), n = 106/47 iEEG contacts for red/gray


groups; (I) Slow wave-spindle couples: P = 0.71 (sync) P = 0.70 (mixed), P = 0.99 (between groups), n = 60/29 iEEG contacts for red/gray groups. N-values differ between panels (g-i) because


channels with zero detection in one of the conditions were excluded. *** is used for P < 0.001, * for P < 0.05. Source data EXTENDED DATA FIG. 6 PHASE-LOCKING CHANGE FOLLOWING


STIMULATION. (A) Spike sorting procedure: (I) top illustration - flexible depth electrodes used for simultaneous recording of iEEG (platinum contacts, blue and black) and unit spiking


activity (recorded on microwires, green). (II) Representative 30-sec example of high-pass filtered (>300 Hz) microwire LFP signal recorded in prefrontal cortex along with threshold for


spike detection (red horizontal line). (III) Screenshot from ‘wave clus’ spike-sorting toolbox91 demonstrating automatic superparamagnetic clustering of wavelet coefficients for 3 clusters.


Left – average waveform for 3 detected clusters, right – each cluster’s waveform (mean and standard deviation) displayed as a heat map91. (IV) Example: Inter spike interval (ISI)


distribution for cluster #1 during pre-stim sleep and post-stim sleep (correlation between distributions is 0.96). (B) TEMPORAL-FIT METHOD FOR SPIKE PHASE DISTRIBUTION: Distribution of


spike-phases from a neural unit recorded in orbitofrontal cortex, phases calculated for MTL iEEG slow-wave signal. (I) Left - before any stimulation block (‘PRE’), right - during the first


‘pause’ block, demonstrating a prolonged effect of sync-stimulation. Colored letters correspond to fitted values in the equation plotted in bottom panel (see below). (II) Left: same


distributions as in (i) in blue (for ‘PRE’), and in red (prolonged condition), overlaid on a polar plot, with mean direction and resultant vector length computed with circstat toolbox93


(Matlab, Mathworks); Right - average and SEM of action potential waveform during the entire intervention session. Calibration bars mark 1 msec and 50 µV. (III) Equation used for fitting the


phase distribution and quantifying locking: specific elements used for calculating phase-locking depth are color-coded and shown also on the example distribution plotted in top panel: red


dashed line, fitted function. a = amplitude/gain, b = preferred phase. c=baseline (mean firing rate/DC). (C) (I) The distribution of depth-lock change in the prolonged condition (pink,


aggregated for n = 65 neural units included in Fig. 3e) is significantly different from a shuffled distribution in which baseline and evaluated condition values are mixed (gray). (II) The


distribution of depth-lock change isn’t significantly different from a bootstrapped distribution calculated based on a sub-set of spikes to test for biases due to firing rate changes


(Kolmogorov-Smirnov two-sample test, P = 0.42) (D) Changes in lock-depth for units outside of MTL are not dependent on selection of baseline. (i) and (ii) compare distributions of change


relative to two possible baselines (shown in gray shade on each timeline): the PRE-block before any stimulation (i) or aggregated 1-min periods preceding all stim blocks (ii). Each violin


plot depicts a distribution of phase-lock changes for a different condition. Pink: prolonged effect (1-min following stim blocks), Yellow: first pause block, Gray: the other panel’s baseline


(gray). (i). There is an increase in phase locking for the prolonged time point relative to the baseline (pink; n = 47 units; P = 0.008), as well as during the entire first pause block


(yellow; n = 27, P = 0.04). (ii) Baseline: gray; n = 35, P = 0.49. Prolonged: pink; n = 32, P = 0.024. First pause block: yellow, n = 25, P = 0.028. P-values are based on Wilcoxon rank-sum


test. Note that inclusion criteria result in slightly different population sizes for each pair of conditions but results are consistent with Fig. 3d, e for all condition-pairs. (E) PHASE


LOCKING CHANGES FOLLOWING MIXED PHASE STIMULATION. Reporting the results of a single stimulation session recorded during a daytime nap. The session included interleaved synched and


mixed-phase stimulation blocks. We did not find elevated phase-locking of single units for this session (n = 14 single units; 6 from MTL) (I) iEEG power spectrum for each vigilance state for


nap session. Blue, NREM sleep. Green, desynchronized states (REM sleep or sporadic wake intervals). Gray, unequivocal wakefulness periods occurring before and after the daytime sleep


session. (II) Average and SEM of MTL probe’s iEEG signal (blue trace, filtered between [0.5–4]Hz to highlight slow-wave activity), time-locked to the positive iEEG peak immediately preceding


stimulation time (t = 0). Box-plots depict the distribution of stimulation delays iEEG slow-wave positive peak for this participant – red is sync-stim blocks and black is mixed-phase stim


blocks. The bounds of the boxes represent the interquartile range and whiskers extend between 1–99 percentiles. (III) Comparison of units’ phase-locking to MTL iEEG pre-stim (left pie


charts) and during stimulation blocks: While units in the MTL were not affected by mixed-phase stimulation, the number of non-significantly phase-locked units outside the MTL increased


during stimulation blocks (an opposite trend than we observed in sync-stimulation sessions – see Fig. 3c). Pink – units phase-locked to MTL iEEG ‘ON’ phase (90–270 degrees); White,


non-significant phase-locking (we did not observe units phase-locked to ‘OFF’ phase in this session). Source data EXTENDED DATA FIG. 7 RIPPLE CHARACTERISTICS IN SPECIFIC MTL REGIONS. (A)


Detected ripples in iEEG electrodes targeting hippocampus (i) Grand average of raw unfiltered iEEG traces (n = 3685 detected ripple events in 12 electrodes/10 patients, mean ± s.e.m.)


aligned to the maximum of the ripple peak during pre-stim epochs. (ii) Average power spectrum of iEEG traces (±1 sec around detected ripples). (iii) Average of ripple-peak-locked TFR


(time-frequency representation, % change from pre-event baseline, color bar on right) highlights the band-limited nature of ripples around 80–120 Hz. (B) Same format as panel a for detected


ripples in iEEG traces of electrodes targeting entorhinal cortex (n = 2646 events in 10 electrodes/7 patients). (C) Same format as panel a for detected ripples in iEEG traces of electrodes


targeting parahippocampal cortex (n = 841 events in 6 electrodes/5 patients). Power spectrum reveals peaks at ~3 Hz and ~14 Hz (fast sleep spindles). Calibration bars mark 100 ms and 30μV


(hippocampus) or 10μV (other MTL regions). Source data EXTENDED DATA FIG. 8 PATHOLOGICAL INTERICTAL EPILEPTIFORM DISCHARGES (IEDS). (A) (I) Grand average of 5819 unfiltered iEEG traces


during pre-stim intervals (mean ± s.e.m) in 33 electrodes with prevalent IED activity based on visual review and neurologist definition (n = 7 participants), aligned to the maximum IED peak


(time 0). Note that these channels were excluded from main analyses based on high rate of abnormal activity. Calibration bars mark 100 ms (x-axis) and 100μV (y-axis). (ii) Average of


IED-locked TFR (% change from pre-event baseline, color bar on right), highlighting the wide-band and high-frequency spectral profile of IEDs. (iii) Grand average iEEG power spectrum around


(±1 s) detected IED events (1–300 Hz, 1 Hz resolution). (B) Effects of stimulation on overnight memory accuracy enhancement (y-axis) vs. change in IED rates (x-axis) do not reveal a


consistent relationship: Recognition memory accuracy enhancement per subject (values as in Fig. 1g) vs. median value of each participant’s distribution of IED change does not show a


significant correlation (Spearman correlation: ρ = −0.12, P = 0.69, n = 12 participants). The distribution for each patient across all iEEG contacts is shown; IED event rates were calculated


over 1-min following stimulations-blocks (yellow shade in top illustration), and normalized relative to an equal time range at the end of each ‘pause’ block (gray shade in top


illustration). Color corresponds to stimulation type, as in Extended Data Fig. 3; black crosses mark the median value for each patient. Source data EXTENDED DATA FIG. 9 COUPLING OF SLEEP


OSCILLATION BETWEEN MTL AND NEOCORTEX. (A) Immediate effect of stimulation on ripple detection. We found a significant reduction, in both stimulation protocols, in ripple event detection


probability on MTL iEEG electrodes calculated during 200 ms following stimulation bursts, relative to sham stimulation points. P-values are reported for a Wilcoxon sign-rank test for each


distribution. Red: MTL contacts in sync-stimulation patients (n = 18 iEEG contacts, P = 0.004); gray: MTL contacts in mixed-phase stimulation patients (n = 8 iEEG contacts, P = 0.01). Only


MTL contacts ipsilateral to the closed-loop input (probe) are included. No significant difference was found between distributions (rank-sum Wilcoxon test. P = 0.7). Note that stimulation was


delivered in neocortical sites, distant from MTL (Fig. 1d). (B) Prolonged effect of stimulation on ripple detection: Event rates were calculated over 1-min following stimulation blocks


(yellow shade in top illustration), relative to an equal time range at the end of each ‘pause’ block (gray shade). Ripple detection rates were stable in MTL channels during ‘pause’ blocks.


Colors as in (A). Wilcoxon sign-rank test for each distribution: Red: n = 18, P = 0.2; Gray: n = 8, P = 0.7. (C) Examples of triple co-occurrences of neocortical slow-waves,


thalamo-cortical-spindles and MTL-ripple events: each example displays simultaneous recordings from a pair of iEEG electrodes in neocortex (black, top row, 0–30 Hz), MTL (blue, middle row,


0–300 Hz) and a ripple band (80–100 Hz) band-pass filtered trace of the MTL iEEG (bottom row). Brown star, detected ripple; Purple, detected slow-wave positive iEEG peak (‘OFF’ period), pink


– detected spindle event. Calibration bars mark 500 ms (x-axis), for visualization purposes iEEG data were z-scored over a 2-sec period plotted in panel. Examples from participants 2 and 14


(D) Prolonged change in MTL ripple-neocortical slow wave co-occurrence incidence: distribution of differences between post-stim period vs pre-stim period is plotted for iEEG channel couples


from each stimulation-mode group. Wilcoxon right-tail signed-rank test: Red: sync-stim, n = 25 iEEG couples, P = 0.0008; gray: mixed-stim, n = 13 iEEG couples, P = 0.36. Wilcoxon rank-sum


test between groups: P = 0.049). (E) Prolonged change in MTL ripple-neocortical slow wave, thalamo-cortical spindle triple co-occurrence incidence: distribution of differences between


post-stim period vs pre-stim period is shown for iEEG channel couples from each stimulation-mode groups. Wilcoxon right-tail signed-rank test: Red: n = 5 sync-stim iEEG couples, P = 0.031;


Gray, n = 7 mixed-stim iEEG couples, P = 0.054, Wilcoxon rank-sum test between groups: P = 0.01). N-values differ between panels (d, e) because channels with zero detection in both


conditions were excluded. *** is used for P < 0.001, * for P < 0.05. Source data EXTENDED DATA FIG. 10 HIGH FREQUENCY SLEEP SPINDLES DETECTED IN CORTICAL CHANNELS. (A) Neocortical iEEG


contacts included in triple-coupling analysis between sleep oscillations in neo-cortex and MTL ripples (n = 41, black), overlaid on a standard (Montreal Neurological Institute) brain


template. (B) High-frequency spindles (above 11 Hz), in iEEG electrodes included in triple-coupling analysis. Top: Grand average of raw unfiltered iEEG traces (n = 3764 events in 41


electrodes from11 patients, mean ± s.e.m) aligned to the maximum of the spindle peak during PRE-stim epochs; bottom: average of spindle-peak-locked TFR (time-frequency representation, %


change from pre-event baseline, color bar on right) highlights the band-limited nature of spindles around 9–16 Hz (marked by dashed lines). (C) Average of slow wave-peak-locked TFR


highlights the increase in spindle-frequency-band (9–15) Hz around slow-wave troughs calculated for slow-waves detected in PRE-stim blocks (same iEEG contacts as in panels a, b). Mean± s.e.m


slow wave trace is superimposed in white (scale shown on righthand y-axis). Source data SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Tables 1–4 REPORTING SUMMARY SOURCE


DATA SOURCE DATA FIG. 1 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA FIG. 2 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA FIG. 3 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA FIG. 4 Statistical source data. SOURCE


DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 1 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 3 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 4 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED


DATA FIG. 5 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 6 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 7 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 8


Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 9 Statistical source data. SOURCE DATA EXTENDED DATA FIG. 10 Statistical source data. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article


is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give


appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in


this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative


Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a


copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Geva-Sagiv, M., Mankin, E.A., Eliashiv, D. _et al._


Augmenting hippocampal–prefrontal neuronal synchrony during sleep enhances memory consolidation in humans. _Nat Neurosci_ 26, 1100–1110 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01324-5


Download citation * Received: 26 August 2021 * Accepted: 06 April 2023 * Published: 01 June 2023 * Issue Date: June 2023 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01324-5 SHARE THIS ARTICLE


Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided


by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative