
Chillsdb 2. 0: individual differences in aesthetic chills among 2,900+ southern california participants
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:

ABSTRACT We significantly enriched ChillsDB, a dataset of audiovisual stimuli validated to elicit aesthetic chills. A total of 2,937 participants from Southern California were exposed to 40
stimuli, consisting of 20 stimuli (10 from ChillsDB and 10 new) presented either in audiovisual or audio-only formats. Questionnaires were administered assessing demographics, personality
traits, state affect, and political orientation. Detailed data on chills responses is captured alongside participants’ ratings of the stimuli. The dataset combines controlled elicitation of
chills using previously validated materials with individual difference measures to enable investigation of predictors and correlates of aesthetic chills phenomena. It aims to support
continued research on the mechanisms and therapeutic potential of aesthetic chills responses. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS CHILLSDB: A GOLD STANDARD FOR AESTHETIC CHILLS STIMULI
Article Open access 20 May 2023 ON THE UNIVERSALITY OF AESTHETIC PREFERENCE AND INFERENCE: A CROSS-CULTURAL (CHINESE–GERMAN) STUDY Article Open access 17 May 2025 COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF
AESTHETIC PREFERENCES IN CHINESE FREEHAND PAINTINGS AMONG CHINESE RETIRED ELDERS BEYOND GENDER Article Open access 04 April 2025 BACKGROUND & SUMMARY Aesthetic chills are a universal
marker of human peak experiences across domains and cultures1,2,3. Characterized by goosebumps and cold shivers down the spine, chills are psychogenic bodily reactions triggered by engaging
with evocative stimuli like music or stories2,4,5. As a conscious, measurable emotion with neural and behavioral correlates, chills show promise for elucidating the relationship between
physiology and affect1,6,7, and for the enhancing positive affect in clinically relevant populations8,9. Studies indicate chills increase altruism, pleasure, attention and memory10,11,
modulate heart rate, pupils, skin conductance and muscle contractions12,13,14, and can generate positive shift in mood and emotion in depression8. Despite the richness of the phenomenon,
most research has been limited to music as the primary stimulus6,7,10. Several databases of stimuli exist to elicit emotions in the laboratory (see review in Table 1). However, none of these
databases is focused exclusively on chills making it difficult for researchers interested in this phenomenon to reliably induce the emotion in populations of interest. To fill this gap,
ChillsDB15 was designed to offer a validated database of audiovisual aesthetic chills stimuli using a novel approach of mining social media content. The original ChillsDB tested 204
potential stimuli across 600 participants. In the current iteration, data was collected from a significantly larger sample of 2,937 participants, all based in Southern California, and
balanced for an even representation of political orientation, sex, age and education level. Participants were presented with 20 stimuli, consisting of a subset of 10 stimuli drawn from the
previously validated set as well as 10 new stimuli. A total of 20 stimuli were presented to participants in two formats-audio only and audiovisual-resulting in 40 total stimulus
presentations with each format represented equally. Additional participant data was collected concerning demographics, personality trait dispositions, and political orientation.
Stimulus-related data included pre- and post-stimulus state affect, valence, and mood ratings as well as questionnaires aimed at characterizing post-stimulus state phenomenology. This
expansion of the dataset allows for a more in-depth exploration of trait, state and demographic factors affecting aesthetic chills, as well as associated phenomenology Fig. 2. METHODS
DATABASE DESIGN We selected 40 stimuli (20 audio and 20 audiovisual) combining a subset from the original Chills DB (N = 10) and a novel subset obtained from additional parsing using the
ChillsDB method and internal polling (N = 10). Each of the 20 stimuli was presented to participants in either two formats - audiovisual or audio-only - in order to compare and test for
differences between the two presentation modalities. ChillsDB stimuli were harvested from online social media platforms, YouTube and Reddit, using a Python-based tool to find stimuli
distributed across social media platforms using breadth-first search algorithm16 (textcolorredNote that all the stimuli used in this experiment were extracted from YouTube). To validate the
stimulus set, we used the Qualtrics online platform to recruit participants. For more details on harvesting, refer to the original ChillsDB article15. PARTICIPANTS A total of 3,259
participants initially took part in the experiment. Participants were recruited and compensated through Qualtrics. Recruitment was performed according to the following quotas: southern
california residents only, approximately equal gender distribution (though gender fluid, nonbinary, and non reporting respondents were also included), and racial/ethnic distributions
approximately conforming to southern california census data. Participants in this study were compensated the equivalent of $12 per hour for their participation in the study. Given initial
piloting suggesting an average of 20–30 minutes for completion, all participants were compensated $8 for their responses to ensure sufficient, consistent compensation. Payment was made via
the Qualtrics platform as follows: Panelists join from a variety of sources. They may be airline customers who chose to join in reward for SkyMiles, retail customers who opted in to get
points at their favorite retail outlet, or general consumers who participate for cash or gift cards, etc. When participants are invited to take a survey, they are informed what they will be
compensated before they enter the survey. We assessed the data validity by adding qualitative questions (“If you experienced chills, please explain what in the video gave you chills and why
you think that is”). These questions were carefully reviewed by two experts and subjects whose response were incoherent with their report were deleted from the dataset. Following this
initial check, we identified 932 participants who reported a non-zero Chills Intensity (Mean I = 19.6; SD = 25) despite reporting that they did not experience chills. This was further
corroborated in their qualitative descriptions, where the majority stated explicitly they did not experience chills. We eliminated all instances (N = 219) where Chills Intensity exceeded 1
standard deviation from the mean (Mean I = 10.3, SD = 20.7). We retained only those participants (N = 656) whose qualitative responses unambiguously confirmed the absence of chills. These
participants were placed in the ‘No Chills’ category and were subsequently omitted from the Chills Intensity analysis. A small number (N) of subjects were eliminated who reported 0 chills
intensity despite reporting chills as this indicated an unreliable responder. Following these data cleaning procedures, the experiment involved a diverse group of 2,937 participants, all of
whom hailed from Southern California (see Tables 2, 3 for the age and education distributions). The gender distribution was fairly balanced, with 54.24% identifying as female and 41.44% as
male. In terms of political affiliation, the largest group identified as Democrats (50.66%), followed by Republicans (21.59%), and Independents (14.81%). Notably, 11.64% of participants did
not specify a political affiliation, while a small proportion (1.19%) identified with other political affiliations. Political Orientation was probed as well and is reported in the database.
In regards to racial identity, the majority of participants (68.44%) identified as White or Caucasian. The second largest racial group was those identifying as Other (11.37%), followed by
American Indian/Native American or Alaska Native (4.97%), and Black or African American (1.46%). A small percentage of participants (0.31%) identified as Asian. Given this broad demographic
range, the present dataset provides a rich, representative sample for examining the phenomena under investigation in the context of Southern California. ETHICS The experiment was conducted
in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration. The protocol was granted an exemption status (Advarra IRB Exemption Pro00068209). All participants gave their voluntary informed consent and
procedures followed the Ethics Code of the American Psychological Association. All participants were informed about the purpose of the research, their right to decline to participate and to
withdraw from the experiment, and the limits of confidentiality. We also provided them with a contact for any questions concerning the research and with the opportunity to ask any questions
regarding the phenomenon under study (aesthetic chills) and receive appropriate answers. DATA RECORDS DATASET STRUCTURE The ChillsDB 2.0. dataset is released under a Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) license on FigShare17. This allows others to freely share and adapt the dataset as long as appropriate credit is given to the original creators by
citing the published paper (Schoeller _et al_., 2023). The dataset is divided into two .csv files available under a CC BY 4.0 license on the associated FigShare17. For a comprehensive
understanding of each column, researchers are advised to refer to the HEADER EXPLANATION FILE 5 (see Table 5). * 1. DATA FILE: This file contains the primary data collected from
participants. * 2. TRAITS QUESTIONNAIRES FILE: This file contains the full questionnaires assessing personality traits including DPES, MODTAS, KAMF, and NEO-FFI that were completed by
participants. * 3. HEADER EXPLANATION FILE: This file provides explanations and more detailed descriptions for each of the columns in the data file. * 4. STIMULI FILE: This file provides
explanations and URL to the stimuli on a private YouTube channel (contact the authors for the.mp4). STIMULI The stimuli are listed in the Stimuli.csv file in the dataset (see also Table 4
and Fig. 1). Due to copyright reasons, all stimuli were stored on a private server. Please contact the authors to be granted access to the.mp4 of the stimuli. We curated 40 stimuli (20 audio
and 20 audiovisual), of which 10 each were directly sourced from the original Chills DB. To compare the efficacy of individual expert recommendations against crowd-sourced ones, we
solicited individual suggestions from the study authors and their extended social networks regarding potential stimuli eliciting aesthetic chills. We then identified overlaps in these
recommendations, which were selected to complete the remaining 10 (N = 10) stimuli for our study. TECHNICAL VALIDATION Participants were recruited for this study through an online platform
(Qualtrics.com) with a focus on individuals residing in Southern California (see Participants section). Before proceeding with the study, participants underwent an initial screening to
confirm their geographical location and provided their informed consent. Participants were then asked to provide basic demographic information including gender, education level, and age.
Additionally, participants were queried about their political orientation and whether they were affiliated with any political party. In order to assess participants’ affective state, they
were prompted to indicate their levels of valence and arousal. Subsequently, participants completed trait questionnaires, including the _Disposition Positive Affect (DEPS)_18, _NEO
Five-Factor Inventory (NEOFFI)_19, _Modified Tellegen Absorption Scale (MODTAS)_20, and _Kama Muta Questionnaire (KAMF)_21. Participants were then randomly assigned to one of 40 stimulus
conditions. After exposure to the assigned stimulus, participants were asked to report their emotional state in terms of valence and arousal once again. They were also asked to indicate how
much they liked the video, whether they had seen the video previously, whether they experienced chills while watching the video, and if so, to rate the frequency and intensity of their
chills. Participants were also asked whether the video reminded them of a personal experience, and if they experienced goosebumps or tears, they were asked to indicate what elicited those
responses. Following the assessment of participants’ immediate responses to the stimulus, they were directed to complete a set of state questionnaires including the _Watts Connectedness
Scale_22, _Ego Dissolution Scale_23, and _Kama Muta Scale_24. Upon completion of the study, participants were thanked for their participation and provided with appropriate remuneration for
their time and effort. The average duration of each experiment was approximately 40 minutes. USAGE NOTES The ChillsDB 2.0 database builds upon the foundation established by the original
ChillsDB, offering an extended scope for aesthetic chills research. Identifying and validating stimuli that can robustly elicit positive affective states as well as phenomenological states
such as ego-dissolution, connectedness and moral elevation, is of value to cognitive and affective neuroscience. Being able to control and manipulate these stimuli in a laboratory setting
provides researchers the experimental control needed to map precise relationships between neural activity and phenomenology. Without standardized, validated stimuli capable of provoking
robust and measurable affective reactions under controlled conditions, researchers lack a solid basis for elucidating the complex neurobiology underlying human emotion and peak experiences.
Indeed, future research should further validate these stimuli by including objective physiological measures in addition to the subjective report that formed the basis of our analysis Table
5. While the initial ChillsDB was centered on identifying and validating chills-eliciting content, ChillsDB 2.0 provides a more comprehensive perspective via a rich set of trait predictors
and state correlates 2. Notably, a leading stimulus in this updated database demonstrates a 0.75 probability of inducing chills across over 70 participants. This database may also have
clinical relevance where this expanded database can contribute to research in areas like depression, where aesthetic chills have been shown to mitigate maladaptive cognition25, improve
hedonic tone26, and aberrant chills response may be a physiological signature of anhedonia8. Chills are also a key target for the development of body-based, interoceptive technologies
enhancing the distinct somatic markers of the emotion9,27. At the social scale, deep analysis of chills-eliciting materials could elucidate themes and narratives centrally important to human
meaning-making3,28. The stimuli that reliably elicit aesthetic chills, as catalogued here, may tap into content that resonates deeply for individuals and cultures?. With a larger
participant sample and detailed individual difference measures, ChillsDB 2.0 may facilitate a deeper understanding of the factors influencing chills experiences, potentially bridging
neuroaesthetics and broader psychological research29. CODE AVAILABILITY The code for parsing YouTube and Reddit networks is available under an MIT license at
https://github.com/ChillsTV/AffectiveStimuliScraper. REFERENCES * Schoeller, F. Knowledge, curiosity, and aesthetic chills. _Frontiers in psychology_ 6, 1546 (2015). Article PubMed PubMed
Central Google Scholar * McCrae, R. R. Aesthetic chills as a universal marker of openness to experience. _Motivation and Emotion_ 31, 5–11 (2007). Article Google Scholar * Schoeller, F.
The shivers of knowledge. _Human and Social Sciences_ https://doi.org/10.1515/hssr-2015-0022 (2015). * McPhetres, J. & Zickfeld, J. H. The physiological study of emotional piloerection:
A systematic review and guide for future research. _International Journal of Psychophysiology_ 179, 6–20, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.06.010 (2022). Article PubMed Google
Scholar * Schoeller, F., Haar, A. J. H., Jain, A. & Maes, P. Enhancing human emotions with interoceptive technologies. _Phys. Life Rev._ 31, 310–319 (2019). Article ADS CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Blood, A. J. & Zatorre, R. J. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. _Proceedings of the
national academy of sciences_ 98, 11818–11823 (2001). Article ADS CAS Google Scholar * Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Longo, G., Cooperstock, J. R. & Zatorre, R. J. The rewarding
aspects of music listening are related to degree of emotional arousal. _PloS one_ 4, e7487 (2009). Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Maes, P.
& Reggente, N. Exploring aesthetic chills as a biomarker in depression. _PsyArXiv_ https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/x26df (2023). * Jain, A., Schoeller, F., Zhang, E. & Maes, P.
Frisson: Leveraging metasomatic interactions for generating aesthetic chills. In _Proceedings of the 2022 International Conference on Multimodal Interaction_, 148–158,
https://doi.org/10.1145/3536221.3556626 (2022). * Fukui, H. & Toyoshima, K. Chill-inducing music enhances altruism in humans. _Frontiers in psychology_ 5, 1215 (2014). Article PubMed
PubMed Central Google Scholar * Sarasso, P. _et al_. Beauty in mind: Aesthetic appreciation correlates with perceptual facilitation and attentional amplification. _Neuropsychologia_ 136,
107282 (2020). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Sumpf, M., Jentschke, S. & Koelsch, S. Effects of aesthetic chills on a cardiac signature of emotionality. _PLoS One_ 10, e0130117
(2015). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Benedek, M. & Kaernbach, C. Physiological correlates and emotional specificity of human piloerection. _Biological psychology_
86, 320–329 (2011). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Zickfeld, J. H., Arriaga, P., Santos, S. V., Schubert, T. W. & Seibt, B. Tears of joy, aesthetic chills and
heartwarming feelings: Physiological correlates of kama muta. _Psychophysiology_ 57, https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13662 (2020). * Schoeller, F. _et al_. Chillsdb: A gold standard for
aesthetic chills stimuli. _Sci Data_ 10, 307 (2023). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Cormen, T. H. 22.2 breadth-first search. (2009). * Schoeller, F., Christov-Moore, L.,
Lynch, C. & Reggente, N. ChillsDB 2.0: Individual Differences in Aesthetic Chills Among 2,900+ Participants in Southern California, _Figshare_,
https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.23935611.v1 (2023). * Dixson, D. D., Anderson, C. L. & Keltner, D. Measuring positive emotions: An examination of the reliability and structural
validity of scores on the seven dispositional positive emotions scales. _Journal of Well-Being Assessment_ 2, 115–133 (2018). Article Google Scholar * Murray, G., Rawlings, D., Allen, N.
B. & Trinder, J. Neo five-factor inventory scores: Psychometric properties in a community sample. _Measurement and evaluation in counseling and development_ 36, 140–149 (2003). Article
Google Scholar * Jamieson, G. A. The modified tellegen absorption scale: A clearer window on the structure and meaning of absorption. _Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis_ 33, 119 (2005). Google Scholar * Zickfeld, J. H. _et al_. Kama muta: Conceptualizing and measuring the experience often labelled being moved across 19 nations and 15 languages.
_Emotion_ 19, 402 (2019). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Watts, R. _et al_. The watts connectedness scale: a new scale for measuring a sense of connectedness to self, others, and world.
_Psychopharmacology_ 239, 3461–3483 (2022). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Nour, M. M., Evans, L., Nutt, D. & Carhart-Harris, R. L. Ego-dissolution and
psychedelics: validation of the ego-dissolution inventory (edi). _Frontiers in human neuroscience_ 10, 269 (2016). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Zickfeld, J. H.,
Schubert, T. W., Seibt, B. & Fiske, A. P. Kammus: A multiplex measure of kama muta. _Emotion_ (2019). * Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Adrien, V. & Maes, P. Aesthetic chills foster
self-acceptance and emotional breakthrough in depression. _PsyArXiv_ https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rhftq (2022). * Jain, A. _et al_. Aesthetic chills cause an emotional drift in valence
and arousal. _Front. Neurosci_. 16, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117 (2023). * Schoeller, F. _et al_. Interoceptive technologies for clinical neuroscience. _PsyArXiv_
https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/sqr6z (2022). * Schoeller, F. The satiation of natural curiosity. _International Journal of Signs and Semiotic Systems_ 5, 27–34,
https://doi.org/10.4018/ijsss.2016070102 (2016). Article Google Scholar * Sarasso, P., Francesetti, G. & Schoeller, F. Editorial: Possible applications of neuroaesthetics to normal and
pathological behaviour. _Front. Neurosci._ 17, 1225308, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2023.1225308 (2023). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Koelstra, S. _et al_. Deap: A
database for emotion analysis;using physiological signals. _IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing_ 3, 18–31, https://doi.org/10.1109/T-AFFC.2011.15 (2012). Article Google Scholar *
McCurrie, C., Crone, D., Bigelow, F. & Laham, S. Moral and affective film set (maafs): A normed moral video database. _PLoS One_ 13, e0206604,
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206604 (2018). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Chen, H., Chin, K. L. & Tan, C. B. Selection and validation of emotional
videos: Dataset of professional and amateur videos that elicit basic emotions. _Data in Brief_ 34, 106662, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2020.106662 (2021). Article CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Negrão, J. G. _et al_. The child emotion facial expression set: A database for emotion recognition in children. _Frontiers in Psychology_ 12, 666245,
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.666245 (2021). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Miolla, A., Cardaioli, M. & Scarpazza, C. Padova emotional dataset of facial
expressions (PEDFE): A unique dataset of genuine and posed emotional facial expressions. _Behavior Research Methods_ 55, 2559–2574, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-022-01914-4 (2022). Article
PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Crosta, A. D. _et al_. The chieti affective action videos database, a resource for the study of emotions in psychology. _Scientific Data_ 7,
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-020-0366-1 (2020). * Abadi, M. K. _et al_. Decaf: Meg-based multimodal database for decoding affective physiological responses. _IEEE Transactions on Affective
Computing_ 6, 209–222, https://doi.org/10.1109/TAFFC.2015.2392932 (2015). Article Google Scholar * Schaefer, A., Nils, F., Sanchez, X. & Philippot, P. Assessing the effectiveness of a
large database of emotion-eliciting films: A new tool for emotion researchers. _Cognition and Emotion_ 24, 1153–1172 (2010). Article Google Scholar * Samson, A. C., Kreibig, S. D.,
Soderstrom, B., Wade, A. A. & Gross, J. J. Eliciting positive, negative and mixed emotional states: A film library for affective scientists. _Cognition and Emotion_ 30, 827–856,
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1031089 (2015). Article PubMed Google Scholar Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Research reported in this publication was supported by Tiny Blue
Dot Foundation and Joy Ventures. AUTHOR INFORMATION Author notes * These authors contributed equally: Felix Schoeller, Leo Christov Moore. AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Institute for Advanced
Consciousness Studies, Santa Monica, California, USA Felix Schoeller, Leo Christov Moore, Caite Lynch & Nicco Reggente * Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Media Lab, Cambridge, USA
Felix Schoeller Authors * Felix Schoeller View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Leo Christov Moore View author publications You can also
search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Caite Lynch View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Nicco Reggente View author publications
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS F.S., L.C.M., C.L. and N.R. conceptualized the study and designed the experiments. FS and L.C.M. analysed the
results. All authors participated equally in writing the manuscript. CORRESPONDING AUTHORS Correspondence to Felix Schoeller or Leo Christov Moore. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS In
the past years, FS co-founded and received compensation from BeSound SAS and Nested Minds LLC. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER’S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons
licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Schoeller, F., Christov Moore, L., Lynch, C. _et al._ ChillsDB 2.0: Individual
Differences in Aesthetic Chills Among 2,900+ Southern California Participants. _Sci Data_ 10, 922 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-023-02816-6 Download citation * Received: 21 August
2023 * Accepted: 01 December 2023 * Published: 21 December 2023 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-023-02816-6 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to
read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing
initiative