
Untangling racism: stress reactions in response to variations of racism against black canadians
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:

ABSTRACT The ability to effectively contend with racism is likely undermined by factors that promote uncertainty regarding racist motives. A national policy of multiculturalism exists in
Canada, which is intended to encourage cultural diversity, but can also serve to mask systemic racism. This may contribute to uncertainty when racism is encountered, with implications for
the appraisals, coping, and stress-related outcomes of ethnoracial minorities. Two studies were conducted to assess Black Canadians’ experiences of racism, and their reactions to variations
in the nature of the experience. A survey study found that Black Canadians’ (_n_ = 158) self-reported past encounters with explicit racism were associated with anger-out coping, which in
turn was related to lower depressive affect. In contrast, ambiguous racism was related to keeping anger in and higher depressive affect; this relation was exacerbated when participants
believed Blacks were held in high regard by others, likely adding uncertainty to interpreting behaviours as racist. An experimental study manipulated the nature of racist cues conveyed in a
videotaped description of the experiences of a Black male interviewee. When the Black interviewee described racist experiences that were ambiguous (vs. explicit) both White (_n_ = 112) and
Black (_n_ = 99) observers were less likely to appraise events as racist, even when physical violence was involved. However, the moment-to-moment distress elicited while listening the
interviewee’s description of their racist experiences was uniquely associated with higher cortisol levels among Blacks (but not Whites), especially when physical violence was present. Taken
together, the findings contribute to our understanding of the processes by which ambiguous racism might operate to undermine the well-being of targeted groups. Moreover, it is suggested that
policies conveying that ethnoracial groups are highly valued (e.g., multiculturalism) might actually exacerbate negative effects of racist experiences, particularly in the absence of an
open discourse acknowledging the presence of systemic racism. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN VICARIOUS RACISM AND PSYCHOACTIVE SUBSTANCE USE DEPEND ON STRENGTH
OF ETHNIC IDENTITY Article Open access 26 July 2024 CARDIOVASCULAR REACTIVITY DURING CONVERSATIONS ABOUT DISCRIMINATION IS BUFFERED BY SOCIAL SUPPORT AMONG U.S. LATINES Article Open access
06 November 2024 EMOTION REGULATION OF SOCIAL EXCLUSION: A CROSS-CULTURAL STUDY Article Open access 14 July 2021 INTRODUCTION Social norms among most Western liberal societies discourage
blatant racist attitudes. Yet, the emergence of the Black Lives Matter movement in the United States has pointed to racism being alive and active. Indeed, under the presidency of Donald
Trump, a visible shift in social norms occurred that condoned expressions of blatant racism and claims of White victimization, which fostered the establishment of social policies that were
clearly racialized (Bloch et al., 2020). At the same time, to the north, in Canada, there were notable assertions that ‘racism does not exist in Canada’ (Lum, 2020), bolstered by the
pervasive belief that Canadians embrace multiculturalism (Dunn and Nelson, 2011). Nonetheless, according to a recent national survey (Neuman, 2019), the majority of Canadians acknowledge
that racism occurs at least occasionally, and that BlacksFootnote 1 and Indigenous peoples (First Nations, Inuit, and Métis) are especially likely to experience racism. There remains,
however, little acknowledgement of structural or systemic racism (i.e., ‘racism without racists’), and even less recognition of the impact of minor day-to-day instances of subtle racism
(e.g., microaggressions), or behaviours that have been described as ‘racism with a smile’ (Jones, 2020; Kunstman et al., 2016). At the same time, these subtle or ambiguous racist acts are
especially likely to undermine the well-being of members of ethnoracial groups (Jones et al., 2016). Based on Canadian census data, there are 1.2M Black Canadians, comprising 3.9% of the
population (Statistics Canada, 2019) (compared to 13.4% of Americans). Approximately 52% of Blacks reside in the province of Ontario. The majority (over 60%) are immigrants, contributing to
more than a doubling of the Black population in Canada since the mid-1990s. While the majority of immigrants came from Caribbean countries (Jamaica, Haiti), this has been shifting with
increased immigration and asylum seeking from African countries (Statistics Canada, 2019). Thus, the explicit subjugation of Blacks is less integrated in Canadian historical and legislative
policies than is the case of Indigenous peoples or in comparison to the experiences of African Americans. Although there exist racially segregated urban areas (Ray and Preston, 2015), this
segregation can be linked to numerous factors, including differences of socio-economic status, and social clustering based on national origins. Canadian identity is commonly tied to
multiculturalism, embraced as a celebration of diversity and tolerance. At the same time, it has been suggested that multiculturalism is a screen behind which many Canadians wilfully
withdraw from a responsibility to challenge contemporary mechanisms supporting racism (Dunn and Nelson, 2011), and indeed, many Canadians seem perplexed by assertions of systemic racism. It
was the goal of the present investigation to assess the implications of experiences of racism among Black Canadians within the unique socio-political context of Canada. Given the historical
roots of the Black population in Canada, together with the principle of multiculturalism that many Canadians believe is inherent to national policies and to their own actions, the experience
and interpretation of racist encounters may be especially ambiguous. Such ambiguity may have consequences for Black Canadians’ ability to contend with such encounters, as well as for their
health and well-being. The present investigation assessed whether Black Canadians’ past experiences of racism were associated with internalizing emotional reactions; such reactions might be
especially pronounced when Black Canadians’ believe that their societal contribution is valued by others. A second study was conducted to explore Blacks’ and Whites’ appraisals of ambiguous
versus explicit racist events targeting a Black victim, and the extent to which exposure to such events elicited differences in emotional (distress, anger) and physiological (cortisol)
reactions. This experimental study systematically distinguished the severity versus ambiguity of racist experiences to determine how these features causally influenced the interpretations
and emotional responses of Black and White Canadians. RACISM AS A STRESSOR The impacts of racism have increasingly been addressed within a stress and coping framework that allows for the
elucidation of the links between racist encounters, and the cognitive, emotional, and physiological responses that promote stress-related illnesses (DeLapp and Williams, 2019). To the extent
that racism is perceived as pervasive, it represents a threat to well-being, eliciting negative emotions, including anger and distress, among targeted individuals (Pieterse et al., 2012;
Schmitt et al., 2014). Concurrently, multiple neurobiological coping processes are triggered that may become overly taxed (allostatic overload; McEwen and Akil, 2020), rendering individuals
vulnerable to the development of stress-related psychological illnesses (e.g., depression, anxiety and posttraumatic stress disorder), as well as numerous physical disorders (e.g., Type II
diabetes, heart disease, and inflammatory related disorders) (Anisman et al., 2018). Under most conditions, and for most people, coping strategies are relatively effective in diminishing the
threat and maintaining well-being. However, an effective coping response to racism may be undermined when ambiguity exists regarding whether the experience constituted racism, as the
targeted individual may be concerned about whether the behaviour was intentionally offensive, whether they merited such treatment, and whether their own behaviour elicited such actions
(Major et al., 2002; Quinn et al., 2014). Under these conditions, the well-being of ethnoracial group members may be harmed to an equal, if not greater, degree than when faced with more
explicit racist acts (Jones et al. 2016; Lui and Quezada, 2019; Matheson et al., 2019). When racism is blatant, it can be labelled as such, thereby reducing uncertainty about how to cope,
justifying feelings of outrage, and mobilizing supports to engage in collective advocacy (Ellemers and Baretto, 2009). In contrast, a meta-analysis revealed consistent relations between
experiencing microaggressions (subtle or ambiguous discrimination) and internalizing affective outcomes (depression, anxiety) (Lui and Quezada, 2019). Not all members of ethnoracial groups
experience such consequences, and some individuals appear to be protected against the negative effects of racism. In this respect, positive regard for one’s ethnic identity conveys a
collective strength that may allow group members to be sensitive to relevant cues (Operario and Fiske, 2001), but at the same time, enables them to ‘call out’ racism when it occurs.
Sensitivity to such cues varies as a function of ethnic group (Forrest-Bank and Jenson, 2015), past experiences with discrimination (Lui, 2020), beliefs about interracial interactions (e.g.,
rejection sensitivity, system justification) (Mercer et al., 2011), and personality differences (e.g., neuroticism) (Lilienfeld, 2017). It appears that numerous contextual and individual
difference factors influence appraisals of a racist encounter, which might, in turn, have implications for well-being. SURVEY STUDY A survey study was conducted to assess the extent to which
explicit versus ambiguous experiences of racism were associated with particular emotional coping responses that, in turn, were linked to depressive affect among Black Canadians. Of the
varied emotions elicited by racism, anger that stems from perceptions of injustice is common (Broudy et al., 2007; Ellemers and Barreto, 2009; Van Zomeren et al., 2004). When anger is
elicited, it can motivate group members to cope by expressing or acting on their outrage (i.e., anger-out), holding the perpetrator group accountable, and mobilizing collective efforts for
change (Brondolo et al., 2005; Ellemers and Barreto, 2009; Gill and Matheson, 2006; Van Zomeren et al., 2004). In contrast, given that ambiguous discrimination tends to be related to
internalizing emotional outcomes (Lui and Quezada, 2019), we anticipated that anger associated with such events might likewise be suppressed (i.e., keeping anger in). Emotional suppression
tends to engender inaction and self-attributions for the negative experience (Gill and Matheson, 2006; Sosoo et al., 2019). Thus, a second goal of the survey study was to assess the unique
relations between explicit and ambiguous experiences of racism with externalizing (anger-out) versus internalizing (anger-in) emotional coping responses, respectively, and the extent to
which these emotions were tied to depressive affect. A third goal of the survey study was to determine whether the appraisal-coping process was influenced by ethnoracial identity. Group
identities are multidimensional with varying implications for how the individual contends with racism (Bombay et al., 2010; Brittian et al., 2015; Sellers et al., 2006). The affective
connection between the individual and their group is reflected in feelings of pride (i.e., private regard for one’s group), as well as in relation to perceptions of the attitudes held by
others toward one’s group (i.e., public regard) (Sellers et al., 1998). Positive affect stemming from high private regard may buffer against negative consequences (Bombay et al., 2010;
Forrest-Bank and Cuellar, 2018), perhaps because the shared experience of racism consolidates group identity and solidarity (Branscombe et al., 1999; Giamo et al., 2012). However, much like
the principles of multiculturalism, high public regard may create uncertainty regarding the interpretation of racist experiences. As a result, negative emotional outcomes were expected to be
more evident among Blacks who perceived high public regard. EXPERIMENTAL STUDY As a self-report survey does not allow for causal conclusions, a second study assessed appraisals, emotional,
and physiological stress reactions to ambiguous and explicit racism by exposing Canadian Blacks and Whites to a manipulation of racist cues in an experimental setting. Although participants
were not directly discriminated against, but rather watched a videotaped interview with the target of racism, observing such interactions can trigger a powerful identity threat response
(Matheson and Anisman, 2012). Reactions to ambiguity are typically examined in the context of microaggressions, which themselves tend to be subtle or mild forms of racism (Hoggard et al.,
2017; Sue, 2010; Tao et al., 2017). Yet, racism is among the top motivators of violent behaviour, accounting for more than half of all recent hate crimes in the United States (Gong et al.,
2017). Similarly, contrary to widely held beliefs within Canada, a Black person is 20 times more likely to be shot and killed by police relative to a White person (Stelkia, 2020). At the
same time, many Whites do not appear to connect how systemic racism can result in violence and even death, and when violent interracial encounters occur, many view them as the actions of a
maverick individual, rather than constituting racism. One objective of the experimental study was to untangle the confound between the ambiguity and severity of racist events by employing a
design wherein ambiguous versus explicit cues were crossed with whether the events were mild (i.e., might be encountered on a day-to-day basis) or included physical violence. In addition to
considering emotional responses to diverse expressions of racism, we assessed the cortisol reponse to these experiences. Typically, stressors promote a cascade of biological changes that
prepare the person to deal with the threat, and to blunt adverse physiological and psychological impacts. In this regard, activation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) processes,
culminating in release of adrenal cortisol, is a functionally adaptive response to stressful stimuli (Sapolsky et al., 2000). The relation between stressful encounters and cortisol
reactivity to laboratory and to natural challenges is particularly pronounced when they elicit shame or were characterized by a lack of control (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004; Michaud et al.,
2008). Thus, to the extent that discrimination is associated with internalizing emotions (such as shame or distress), elevated cortisol might be expected to occur. Cortisol reactivity may be
further influenced by prior stressor experiences. Chronic stressors such as racism may result in down-regulated cortisol reactivity, perhaps as an adaptive response to prevent downstream
negative outcomes (Matheson and Anisman, 2012). This said, upon exposure to salient reminders of these stressors, a stronger cortisol response may be elicited, perhaps owing to the
sensitization of processes associated with HPA functioning (Anisman et al., 2018). At the same time, past racism experiences can mobilize ‘accumulated resources’ that enable ethnoracial
group members to appraise discrimination experiences as such, contribute to effective coping, and buffer against stress-related outcomes (DeLapp and Williams, 2019). Thus, the experimental
study assessed emotional (anger, distress) and cortisol responses elicited by ambiguous or explicit racist cues as a function of Black Canadians’ past experiences of racism. Racism is an
intergroup experience, not constrained to the impacts on targeted ethnoracial minorities. In this regard, witnesses to discrimination may report negative emotional reactions (Schmader et
al., 2012), particularly in response to explicit racism (Tao et al., 2017; Torres et al., 2020), although other studies reported more apathetic reactions (Karmali et al., 2017). As social
norms encourage multiculturalism and anti-racism, sincerity may or may not be at the foundation of liberal Whites’ responses (Radke et al., 2020; Torres et al., 2020). Indeed, ‘modern
racism’ (McConahay, 1986) is expressed by attributing differential values and attitudes that serve to justify group inequities, or advocating that enough change has occurred to alleviate
them. As a result, when racial injustices are made salient, modern racist biases might elicit indifference to the target group’s suffering (Passini, 2019). Thus, the concurrence between
Canadian Whites’ self-report appraisals and indicators of their reactions that are less amenable to response biases (namely their moment-to-moment emotions and cortisol reactivity to racist
cues), and whether these processes were moderated by modern racist beliefs might provide insight into Whites’ reactions to racism. METHODS SURVEY STUDY PARTICIPANTS AND PROCEDURES Black
participants (_n_ = 158) were recruited through advertisements posted on various public websites, newspapers and community flyers, and signs posted on public bulletin boards of community
centres, service organizations, and workplaces. They were invited to participate in a survey regarding Canadian Life Experiences, either online or in paper format exchanged by mail. Upon
submission of their survey responses, participants were provided a written debriefing and were entered into an instant draw lottery for a $10 gift certificate, as well as a $500 lottery held
at the end of data collection. Both the survey and experimental studies were approved by the research ethics boards at Carleton University and Wilfrid Laurier University. The majority of
respondents were female (_n_ = 106, 67.5%; males _n_ = 51, 32.3%; 1 person did not disclose), and ages ranged from 16 to 65 years (_M_ = 26.03, _SD_ = 9.39). Most were Canadian citizens
(80.4%) or landed immigrants (7.6%), although some were in Canada on temporary (student) visas (12.0%). The majority (80.3%) had some undergraduate or college education. MEASURES The
Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire (Contrada et al., 2001) comprised 22 items assessing experiences of _explicit_ day-to-day racism (e.g., ethnic slurs, insults, denial of equal
treatment, actual or threatened harm), and a further 22 items were included to assess _ambiguous_ racism encounters (e.g., “Sometimes I’m not sure if what I’m seeing or hearing is racist”).
Respondents rated how often they had encountered each experience over the past 3 months from 1 (never happened) to 7 (happened very often), and ratings were averaged to reflect perceptions
of explicit (Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.93) and ambiguous racism (Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.94). The Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity (Sellers et al., 1998) assessed private regard (six items,
e.g., “I feel good about Black people”) and public regard (six items, e.g., “In general, others respect Black people”). Agreement with each statement was indicated using a −3 (strongly
disagree) to 3 (strongly agree) rating scale, and average ratings for each subscale formed the basis of indices of private (Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.77) and public regard (Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.83).
Two subscales from the Multidimensional Anger Inventory (Siegel, 1986) were administered, namely anger-out (e.g., “When I’m angry with someone, I let that person know”) (2 items, _r_ = 0.56)
and anger-in (e.g., “I harbour grudges that I don’t tell anyone about”) (4 items, Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.72). Participants rated how much each statement applied to them on a scale ranging from
1 (not at all) to 5 (completely). Depressive affect was assessed using the 21-item version of the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961). For each question, participants chose from
one of four responses (coded 0–3), with each response reflecting increasing symptom severity. Summed scores could range from 0 to 63 (Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.88). EXPERIMENTAL STUDY PARTICIPANTS
Black (_n_ = 99) and White (_n_ = 112) participants were recruited from a booth set up at the Ontario Science Centre (_n_ = 47) in Toronto, Canada, as well as through postings at a Canadian
university (_n_ = 164). The study was described as concerning reactions to various experiences in the Canadian workplace. University participants were paid $10 or given experimental credit
for their participation. The age ranges of Black (_M_age = 20.7, _SD_ = 4.80; range 16–42 years) and White (_M_age = 22.0, _SD_ = 7.04; range 16–47 years) participants were equivalent. The
majority of both Black (55 females, 41 males; 3 unspecified) and White participants (78 females, 34 males) were female. Most had completed high school diplomas (Black _n_ = 47; White _n_ =
45) or 1–3 years of university (Black _n_ = 38; White _n_ = 49), although a few had completed either an undergraduate or graduate degree (Black _n_ = 9; White _n_ = 10). PROCEDURES Because
cortisol varies in a diurnal fashion, participants were tested between 11:30 and 17:00 hours. They were asked to not eat, drink, or smoke for 30 min prior to the study. At the outset of the
session, participants were provided with an overview of the procedures, written informed consent was obtained, and after completing background information measures, they were randomly
assigned to watch one of five videotaped interviews that varied in the severity and explicitness of racism. The videos were linked (via the computer) to a wireless interactive hand-held dial
through which participants conveyed their moment-to-moment emotional distress to statements made by the interviewee. At three timepoints during the videos, participants appraised the events
depicted (presence of discrimination, seriousness of impact). Following the videos, they completed a measure of feelings of anger, and past discrimination experiences (Blacks) or modern
racist attitudes (Whites). Finally, participants were debriefed, and provided with contact numbers should they experience any distress or have any human rights concerns, and provided with
experimental credit or payment. VIDEO MATERIALS Each of the videos depicted an interview in the context of a local daily news programme that was conducting a series on “Working in Ontario:
Meeting employee needs”. The videos were professionally created using well-rehearsed non-professional actors. In all five conditions, a white male host interviewed a Black male in his
mid-20s. The same actors were used in all conditions. All of the videos began with the host asking questions about the guest interviewee’s workplace. The first segment (2.5 min) was the same
across all conditions, wherein the guest described his career history working up to his current position as a Graphics Designer for a large firm. Responses were fact-based, along with
comments that depicted the workplace environment as mildly stressful (e.g., small office, crowded conditions). The second segment of the video interview (6 min) built on these features to
describe mildly stressful social interactions, as well as workplace limitations, such as challenges in career advancement. Descriptions of these events varied to reflect no racism, explicit
mild racism, or ambiguous mild racism. For example, in all videos, the interviewee described how he felt when he was successfully promoted. In the no racism video, the interviewee reported
“I still wonder whether people think that I really deserved the job—you know, some of us just don’t have the self-confidence to promote ourselves well”, whereas in the explicit mild racism
video, he indicated “I still hear people say that the only reason I got the job was because they needed to promote a Black to avoid having to implement an affirmative action quota plan.” In
the ambiguous racism video, the interviewee expresses uncertainty about others’ attributions for his success, “I still wonder whether people think that I got the job because they know I was
the best person, or whether they think it was because our firm had to promote a Black to avoid having to implement an affirmative action quota plan.” Finally, the third segment of the video
(3.5 min) asked the interviewee questions about his encounters with his colleagues outside the workplace. There were five variations of this segment of the video. In all of five conditions,
he describes an experience of going to a local bar after work. The description includes tensions in organizing the group to get to the bar, the rudeness of his colleagues to the bartender,
and their negative reaction to him when he suggested they alter their behaviour, and his decision to leave the bar. In the no racism, explicit and ambiguous mild racism conditions the
interviewee’s description ended here. To introduce physical violence that was ambiguous, in a fourth condition (that built from the no racism scenario in segments 1 and 2), the interviewee
describes his decision to leave the situation, “I went out to wait for the cab, and that’s where he was, waiting for me. I tried to turn to go back into the bar, but he blocked my way. I
turned to walk away, and he grabbed me from behind, and said ‘no-one talks to me like that in front of my buddies. You gotta know your place, and I’m going make sure you never forget.’ He
jerked me by the throat and threw me to the ground. But then I was lucky—I heard a siren start right behind us, and he ran. I had blood running down my face from a cut I got from broken
glass on the ground. The cops asked me whether I knew the guy, but I was afraid to turn him in. I had to work with him, and it might have ended my career to start trouble.” In the fifth
condition (which built on the mild explicit racism reported in segment 2), the violence was described as explicitly racist through the inclusion of explicit language, and highlighting that
these were not the isolated actions of a single individual“….the guy that I’d confronted put his arms around my neck from behind, and said ‘no Black boy gets away with talking to me like
that in front of my buddies. You gotta know your place, and I’m going make sure you never forget.’ He jerked me by the throat and threw me to the ground. The other guys started to laugh. But
then I was lucky—I heard a siren start right behind us, and they all ran. I had blood running down my face from a cut I got from broken glass on the ground. The cops asked me what I did to
start the fight. I started to protest, but I was afraid to turn in the other guys. It might have ended my career to say anything.” SALIVARY CORTISOL Saliva collection comprised participants
placing a piece of dental cotton in their cheek for a 2-min period. Once the cotton was thoroughly wet, it was placed in a test-tube for subsequent analysis. This procedure was conducted
three times: baseline levels were assessed 15 min after arrival at the test session, but before watching a video; and then again 10 and 20 min after watching the video. Based on our earlier
studies, these times represent peak saliva cortisol levels following a stressor (Matheson and Anisman, 2009). Cortisol levels were determined, in duplicate, by means of a solid phase
radio-immuno assay using 125I kits (ICN Biomedicals Inc., CA). The intra- and extra-assay variability was <10%. Time of day at which samples were collected was not significantly related
to cortisol levels at any time point, nor were there significant differences in time of day across racist cues conditions or ethnic group. In addition, among the female participants, current
use of oral contraceptives was not related to levels of cortisol at any of the three time points. For the purposes of analyses, area under the curve with respect to zero was calculated for
each participant (formula 3, Pruessner et al., 2003). MEASURES Appraisals of the experiences the interviewee described in the videos were assessed after each of the three segments. This
comprised two items assessing whether his experiences constituted discrimination and participants’ confidence in their determination of whether the events were due to discrimination, ranging
from –3 (not at all) to +3 (definitely) (_r_s = 0.40, 0.81, 0.79, _p_s < 0.001, between the two items at each measurement time, respectively). The perceived seriousness of the situation
was assessed by two items, including ratings (from –3 to +3) of ‘how serious do you think the events that have been described so far are in terms of their impact on the employee’s career’
and on his ‘psychological well-being’ (_r_s = 0.37, 0.38, 0.40, _p_s < 0.001 for each time point, respectively). The videos were synchronized with a wireless interactive dial through
which participants conveyed ongoing emotional reactions (how distressed or anxious they felt on a scale of 0–10) to statements made by the interviewee. Moment-to-moment ratings were captured
at 1 second intervals. As a result, there were about 150 ratings for the first section of the video, 370 for the second, and 200 for the third. For each of the three video segments,
distress ratings were averaged to provide an index of distress for each segment. After watching the videos, participants’ feelings of anger were assessed using five mood adjectives (angry,
annoyed, enraged, infuriated, irritated) with ratings from 0 (not at all) to 6 (extremely) (Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.90). To assess Black participants’ experiences of discrimination over the past
6 months, they responded to eight questions, each of which tapped into the dimensions reflected in the Personal Experiences of Discrimination Questionnaire (e.g., ‘How often have you been
called names or heard offensive comments because of your ethnic group membership, spoken either in your presence or behind your back?’). Responses ranged from 1 (never) to 7 (very often) and
were averaged to reflect past discrimination experiences (_M_ = 3.06, _SD_ = 1.26; Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.84). White participants responded to four questions selected from a measure of modern
sexist biases in Canada (Tougas et al., 1995), modified to refer to Blacks (e.g., ‘Blacks shouldn’t push themselves where they are not wanted’). Ratings of agreement, from 0 (strongly
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), were averaged to provide an index of modern racist attitudes (_M_ = 0.43, _SD_ = 1.26; Cronbach’s _α_ = 0.60). RESULTS SURVEY STUDY DESCRIPTIVES Black
participants reported experiencing both explicit (_M_ = 2.58, _SD_ = 1.01) and especially ambiguous (_M_ = 3.03, _SD_ = 1.23) forms of racism, _t_(140) = −4.97, _p_ < 0.001, _r_ = 0.54,
_p_ < 0.001. Both explicit, _r_ = −0.26, _p_ = 0.002, and ambiguous, _r_ = −0.25, _p_ = 0.003, experiences of racism were associated with lower public regard, but neither type of racism
was related to private regard, _r_s = −0.08, ns. In addition, males indicated more frequent encounters with explicit discrimination (_M_ = 2.81, _SD_ = 1.19) than did females (_M_ = 2.44,
_SD_ = 0.88), _F_(1,140) = 4.26, _p_ = 0.041. There were no gender differences in experiences of ambiguous racism, private or public regard, anger-out, anger-in, or depressive affect. UNIQUE
EFFECTS OF EXPLICIT AND AMBIGUOUS RACISM Multiple regression analyses were conducted to assess the relations between explicit and ambiguous racism experiences (entered simultaneously) and
each of anger-out and anger-in coping strategies, and depressive affect (outcomes). As expected, explicit racism experiences were associated with greater anger-out, _r_ = 0.16, _b_ = 0.31,
_se_ = 0.11, _p_ = 0.006, with ambiguous racism appearing to serve as a suppressor variable, _r_ = −0.08, _b_ = −0.25, _se_ = 0.11, _p_ = 0.024; full model _F_(2,134) = 5.81, _p_ = 0.004,
_R_2 = 0.080. Thus, when controlling variance associated with ambiguity, the relation between explicit experiences and anger-out was strengthened. In contrast, ambiguous racism was related
to greater anger-in, _r_ = 0.28, _b_ = 0.22, _se_ = 0.07, _p_ = 0.002, whereas explicit racism was not, _r_ = 0.10, _b_ = −0.06, _se_ = 0.09, _p_ = 0.496; full model _F_(2,133) = 4.37, _p_ =
0.015, _R_2 = 0.062. When predicting depressive affect, explicit racism served as the suppressor variable, _r_ = 0.14, _b_ = −0.21, se = 0.86, _p_ = 0.810, enhancing the relation between
ambiguous encounters and greater depressive affect, _r_ = 0.29, _b_ = 1.90, se = 0.68, _p_ = 0.006; full model _F_(2,109) = 5.08, _p_ = 0.008, _R_2 = 0.085. In effect, when the contribution
of ambiguity was isolated (by controlling explicit racism), ambiguity was more strongly related to depressive affect. MODERATING ROLE OF PRIVATE AND PUBLIC REGARD FOR BLACKS To assess
whether the relations between racism and anger coping were moderated by private or public regard, regressions were conducted using PROCESS macro (Version 3.3) model 1, centering continuous
predictor variables prior to calculating the interaction (Hayes, 2017). These analyses indicated that over and above the relation with explicit racism, _b_ = 0.21, _se_ = 0.09, _p_ = 0.026,
higher private regard predicted greater anger expressed outwardly, _b_ = 0.36, _se_ = 0.12, _p_ = 0.003. The interaction between explicit racism and private regard was not significant, _b_ =
−0.03, _se_ = 0.10, _p_ = 0.791; full model _F_(3,134) = 4.42, _p_ = 0.005, _R_2 = 0.090. Perceived public regard was not associated with anger-out. Whereas perceptions of public regard for
Blacks were not directly related to anger-in, _b_ = −0.08, _se_ = 0.06, _p_ = 0.161, public regard moderated the relation between ambiguous racism and anger-in, _b_ = 0.11, _se_ = 0.04, _p_
= 0.010; full model _F_(3,132) = 6.38, _p_ < 0.001, _R_2cha = 0.127. As seen in Fig. 1, simple slopes analyses indicated that when others were perceived to view Blacks negatively (low
public regard), ambiguous racist experiences were not associated with anger-in, _b_ = 0.06, _se_ = 0.07, _p_ = 0.402. In contrast, as public regard for Blacks increased, so too did the
association between ambiguous racism and holding anger in, _b_ = 0.34, _se_ = 0.09, _p_ < 0.001. Private regard was not associated with anger-in. MEDIATING ROLE OF ANGER COPING IN THE
RELATIONS BETWEEN RACISM AND DEPRESSIVE AFFECT To determine whether anger mediated the relations between experiences of racism and depressive affect, the PROCESS macro applying model 4
(Hayes, 2017) was used. These analyses were followed up to determine whether private or public regard moderated the mediated models using PROCESS model 7. The macros used bootstrapping
procedures with 5000 resamples to establish the 95% confidence intervals (CI) to assess significance of the path coefficients. The total effect of explicit racism in relation to depressive
affect was not significant, _c_ = 1.27, _se_ = 0.79, CI.95 [−0.29, 2.84], but with the inclusion of anger-out as a mediator, a direct effect of explicit racism was apparent, _c’_ = 1.75,
_se_ = 0.78, CI.95 [0.20, 3.31]. The mediated path between explicit racism and depressive affect through anger-out was also significant, _ab_ = −0.48, _se_ = 0.27, CI.95 [−1.10, −0.06]. In
essence, with anger-out controlled, explicit racism was related to greater depressive affect. Conversely, when explicit racism was associated with greater expression of anger, lower
depressive affect was reported (mediation). Private regard did not moderate this mediated model. The total effect of experiencing ambiguous racism in relation to greater depressive affect
was significant, _c_ = 1.82, _se_ = 0.57, CI.95 [0.69, 2.95]. With the inclusion of anger-in as a mediator, the direct effect remained significant, _c'_ = 1.61, _se_ = 0.57, CI.95
[0.47, 2.75], and the mediated effect through anger-in was marginally significant, _ab_ = 0.21, _se_ = 0.14, CI.90 [0.02, 0.48]. Public regard significantly moderated this mediated model,
Index = 0.15, _se_ = 0.09, CI.95 [0.002, 0.36], in that, the mediated pathway between ambiguous racism and depressive affect through anger-in was evident at higher, _ab_ = 0.27, _se_ = 0.14,
_p_ = 0.012, but not lower, _ab_ = 0.02, _se_ = 0.08, _p_ = 0.826, levels of public regard. SUMMARY OF SURVEY RESULTS Consistent with past research (Lui and Quezada, 2019), experiences of
explicit racism were associated with expressing anger, whereas ambiguous experiences were related to suppressing anger. Moreover, when the ambiguity was controlled, the relation between
explicit racism and anger-out was strengthened (i.e., the suppressor effect), and higher private regard (Black pride) was additively related to anger-out. To the extent that explicit racism
was related to anger being expressed, lower depressive affect was evident. This said, when anger-out was controlled, explicit racism was associated with higher depressive affect. In
contrast, the relation between ambiguous racism and anger-in remained after controlling the experience of discrimination itself (explicit racism) and was exacerbated when combined with the
additional uncertainty associated with thinking that others held Blacks in high regard. This suggests that ambiguity in itself represents a threat to individuals’ well-being, potentially
undermining effective strategies to confront the situation, and hence higher depressive affect. EXPERIMENTAL STUDY As seen in Table 1, the experimental design involved five conditions in
which racist cues were manipulated in the context of a videotaped interview with a Black male interviewee. To assess participants’ responses to racist cues, 2 (ethnoracial group: Black vs.
White)×5 (racist cues: no racism, ambiguous racism (violent vs. mild), and explicit racism (violent vs. mild))×3 (video segment) mixed measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted.
Simple effects analyses involved orthogonal contrasts comparing (i) the no racism condition to the four racism conditions, (ii) ambiguous vs. explicit racism, and (iii) violent vs. mild
events within each of the ambiguous and explicit racism conditions. Female participants appraised the videos overall as being more discriminatory (_M_ = 1.69, _SD_ = 1.76) than did males
(_M_ = 1.17, _SD_ = 2.14), _F_(1,201) = 4.57, _p_ = 0.034, and also expressed greater distress (_M_ = 2.70, _SD_ = 2.43) than did males (_M_ = 1.98, _SD_ = 2.10), _F_(1,201) = 5.97, _p_ =
0.015. Gender differences were not moderated by ethnoracial group nor racial cues. EFFECTS OF RACIAL CUES ON APPRAISALS As expected, appraisals of whether the events described in the video
comprised _discrimination_ varied across video conditions, _F_(8,400) = 27.91, _p_ < 0.001, _η_2 = 0.358. Ethnoracial group further moderated the effects of racist cues on discrimination
appraisals, _F_(4,200) = 3.40, _p_ = 0.01, _η_2 = 0.064. As seen in Fig. 2, there were no differences in appraisals at the outset of the video (recall that this segment was identical across
conditions). The second segment introduced _ambiguous_ mild racism in the relevant condition, and _explicit_ mild racism in two of the conditions. Indeed, greater discrimination was
perceived in these three conditions, in contrast to those in which racism was not yet introduced, _t_(204) = 10.95, _p_ < 0.001. In addition, Whites perceived these events to be less
discriminatory than did Blacks, _F_(1,200) = 14.34, _p_ < 0.001, _η_2 = 0.067. Further seen in Fig. 2, by the third segment, in which conditions involving physical violence were
introduced, both Blacks and Whites were more likely to perceive the conditions involving explicit racist cues as more discriminatory than when cues were ambiguous, _t_(200) = 7.13, _p_ <
0.001. Explicit racist events were perceived as being equally discriminatory, irrespective of the presence of violence, _t_(200) = 0.53, _p_ = 0.60. When cues were ambiguous, participants
were _less_ likely to appraise the violent situation as discriminatory, _t_(200) = −5.92, _p_ < 0.001; this interacted with ethnoracial group, _t_(200) = 2.28, _p_ = 0.008, with Whites
being particularly unlikely to appraise ambiguous violence as discriminatory. Appraisals of the _seriousness_ of the impact of the events also changed over the course of the video as a
function of racist cues, _F_(8,400) = 5.16, _p_ < 0.001, _η_2 = 0.093. Although Black participants were more likely to perceive a serious impact on the interview than were White
participants, _F_(1,200) = 4.26, _p_ = 0.021, _η_2 = 0.038, ethnoracial group did not interact with racist cues, _η_2s < 0.02. As depicted in Fig. 3, the introduction of either mild
ambiguous or explicit racist cues resulted in perceiving the situation as more serious, _t_(207) = 5.92, _p_ < 0.001. In the third segment of the video, explicit racism was perceived to
have a greater impact on the target compared to when racism was ambiguous, _t_(205) = 3.30, _p_ = 0.001. Moreover, although differences in the perceived impact of violent versus mild
ambiguous events were not significant, _t_(205) = 0.21, _p_ = 0.831, the combination of explicit racism and violence was appraised as having a particularly serious impact, _t_(205) = 2.36,
_p_ = 0.019. EFFECTS OF RACIAL CUES ON EMOTIONAL REACTIONS An ANOVA conducted on average moment-to-moment ratings of distress within each the three video segments indicated a three-way
interaction between ethnoracial group, racist cues, and video segment, _F_(8,398) = 2.59, _p_ = 0.009, _η_2 = 0.049. As expected, distress was virtually absent throughout the first segment
of the videos. Through the second segment, participants expressed greater distress as racist cues emerged, _t_(206) = 5.14, _p_ < 0.001. However, as seen in Fig. 4, White participants
indicated equal distress in response to ambiguous versus explicit racist cues, whereas Black participants expressed significantly greater distress when the cues were explicit, _t_(206) =
3.90, _p_ < 0.001. Likewise, in the third segment, although greater distress was conveyed by both Blacks and Whites when racist cues were present, _t_(203) = 3.38, _p_ = 0.001, Whites’
distress did not vary as a function of the nature of the racist cues, _F_(4,106) = 1.42, _p_ = 0.233. Among Blacks (Fig. 4), greater distress was expressed in response to explicit racism,
irrespective of whether the events included violence, _t_(93) = 3.71, _p_ < 0.001. An ANOVA on ratings of anger taken at the end of the videos indicated that, overall, Blacks expressed
greater anger (_M_ = 2.67, _SD_ = 1.76) than did Whites (_M_ = 2.02, _SD_ = 1.60), _F_(1,200) = 8.84, _p_ = 0.003, _η_2 = 0.042. In addition, there was a main effect of racist cues,
_F_(4,200) = 13.38, _p_ < 0.001, _η_2 = 0.211, which was not moderated by ethnoracial group, _F_ < 1. As seen in Fig. 5, the presence of racist cues elicited greater anger, _t_(205) =
5.97, _p_ < 0.001, particularly in response to explicit (relative to ambiguous) racism, _t_(205) = 4.12, _p_ < 0.001, irrespective of whether the events were mild or violent.
MODERATORS OF RESPONSES TO RACIST CUES The moderating roles of individual difference factors (past experiences of racism among Blacks; modern racist attitudes among Whites) on the effects of
racist cues on appraisals and emotional reactions at the end of the video were assessed by means of multiple regression analyses conducted separately for each ethnoracial group. Among
Blacks, there was a significant interaction between past racism and racist cues in the videos on appraisals of whether the events were discriminatory, _F_(4,88) = 2.85, _p_ = 0.028, _R_2cha
= 0.056. Simple effects analyses (Fig. 6) indicated that when fewer past experiences of racism were reported, appraisals of discrimination were greatest when racist cues were explicit,
_F_(4,88) = 19.85, _p_ < 0.001. As past experience with racism increased, Black participants were more likely to regard ambiguous events as constituting discrimination, _F_(4,88) = 7.39,
_p_ < 0.001. Past racism experiences did not moderate the effects of racist video cues on the perceived seriousness of the events depicted, distress, or anger. Whites’ appraisals of the
events in the videos were moderated by modern racist attitudes, _F_(4,99) = 6.78, _p_ < 0.001, _R_2cha = 0.072. Simple effects analyses indicated that, as seen in Fig. 7 (left), those who
were less racist were more likely to perceive discrimination when it was explicit or ambiguous and mild, _F_(4,99) = 52.97, _p_ < 0.001. Whites expressing higher modern racism were less
likely to appraise ambiguous events as constituting discrimination, _particularly_ when violence was involved, _F_(4,99) = 27.55, _p_ < 0.001. This moderating effect was also observed in
relation to Whites’ appraisals of the seriousness of the impact of the events depicted, _F_(4,99) = 4.36, _p_ = 0.003, _R_2cha = 0.126. Simple effects analyses (Fig. 7, right) indicated that
those who were less racist were more likely to perceive the events in all four racist conditions (relative to no racism) as having serious negative implications for the target, _F_(4,99) =
6.63, _p_ < 0.001. In contrast, those expressing higher modern racism only appraised explicit violent racism as having more serious consequences, _F_(4,99) = 4.02, _p_ = 0.005. Modern
racism did not alter patterns associated with distress or anger among Whites. CORTISOL REACTIVITY As it was expected that cortisol variations should correspond with levels of distress (and
possibly anger), hierarchical regression analyses were conducted, wherein the area under the curve (AUC) associated with cortisol levels was regressed onto distress or anger (independent
variables), followed by racist cues condition (four coded variables reflecting the orthogonal contrasts) and ethnoracial group (moderating variables), and the 2-way and 3-way interactions on
subsequent steps. A significant interaction between distress and ethnoracial group on cortisol AUC was demonstrated, _F_(1,178) = 5.57, _p_ = 0.019, _R_2cha = 0.027, as was the interaction
between distress and racist cues condition, _F_(4,174) = 3.67, _p_ = 0.007, _R_2cha = 0.067. Among Blacks, greater distress was associated with higher cortisol AUC, _b_ = 0.153, _se_ =
0.038, _p_ < 0.001, whereas among Whites, this relation was not significant, _b_ = 0.007, _se_ = 0.052, _p_ = 0.889. Further, among Blacks, the positive relation between distress and
cortisol AUC was especially pronounced in the two conditions conveying physical violence (_r_ambiguous = 0.68, _r_explicit = 0.72, _p_s < 0.001 vs. conditions with no violence, _r_ =
0.40, _p_ < 0.001), whereas among Whites, the only condition in which this relation reached significance was when physical violence occurred and racist cues were ambiguous (_r_ambiguous
violence = 0.65, _p_ = 0.016, _r_explicit violence = −0.04, _p_ = 0.894, _r_no violence = 0.01, _p_ = 0.889). Anger was not related to cortisol AUC, irrespective of ethnoracial group or
racist cues. Past experiences of racism among Blacks was not a significant moderating variable, nor was modern racism among Whites. DISCUSSION Given a national policy of multiculturalism in
Canada, coupled with the demographic characteristics of the resident Black population, it was of interest to assess the implications of experiences of racism for Black Canadians’ emotional
and cognitive reactions to such encounters, as well as for their well-being. Experiences of racism are thought to affect mental health among members of ethnoracial groups by way of
heightened stress responses. Less clear are the contextual factors that help individuals to make sense of racist encounters and determine effective strategies to contend with them (Lui,
2020; Pieterse and Powell, 2016). Indeed, as previously observed (Jones et al., 2016; Lui and Quezada, 2019; Matheson et al., 2019; Tao et al., 2017), explicit and ambiguous racism in the
present investigation were differentially related to emotional coping and with depressive affect. However, the interplay between these types of racism and their implications for coping
strategies and stress outcomes was complex. Typically, explicit and ambiguous racism are treated as independent constructs (or sometimes as being at opposite ends of a continuum), when in
fact, as in the present study, not only were reports of such encounters correlated, but their joint consideration revealed unique patterns of relations. In particular, past experiences of
explicit racism were not related to depressive affect among Blacks. But, together with greater private regard (i.e., pride in their Black identity), explicit racism was associated with
greater expression of anger (anger-out), which in turn, was related to lower depressive affect. These relations are in keeping with the view that group members who cope by expressing their
outrage are more likely to engage in collective actions to address racism (Becker et al., 2011; Brondolo et al., 2005; Ellemers and Barreto, 2009). In this sense, anger expression may be a
highly adaptive coping response to discrimination (Matheson and Anisman, 2009). This said, the propensity to express anger that was associated with explicit racism masked a _positive_ link
between explicit racism and depressive affect that was only evident when this anger coping strategy was statistically controlled. This underlying link between racism and depressive affect
may render the individual more vulnerable to stress-related disturbances, particularly when feelings of anger or hostility are prolonged (Busch et al., 2017; Pittman, 2011). Explicit racism
also played a suppressor role in the relation between ambiguous racism and depressive affect. In this regard, once the shared variance between the two types of racism was accounted for,
ambiguous racist experiences were _more strongly_ associated with depressive affect. A history of ambiguous racism was further related to greater anger-in, which in turn, predicted greater
depressive affect. Ambiguity may create uncertainty not only for how to interpret the event, but also with respect to how the perpetrator may react to labelling the interaction as racist.
Perhaps for this reason, when Black Canadians perceived high public regard, ambiguous racism was even more strongly associated with supressing anger. Given the socio-political context,
Blacks experiencing ambiguous forms of racism might be especially concerned that the perpetrator would react to confrontation by claiming that their actions were misperceived, or that the
target is simply a complainer (Garcia et al., 2010). Indeed, there is much anecdotal evidence justifying such a concern (Donovan, 2020). Moreover, suggesting that ambiguous actions are
racist could instigate a cyclical interaction wherein the target’s claims are responded to with further instances of negative behaviour on the part of the perpetrator (Jones et al., 2017).
To the extent that anger is experienced but remains unexpressed, a ‘simmering resentment’ might build that could motivate collective action at a later time, as clarity regarding systemic
racism evolves. Of particular interest in the experimental study was to disentangle a common confound between the ambiguity and the severity of racist events. The majority of research
assessing ambiguous racism has focused on mild microaggressions, finding similar, if not stronger relations to stress-related outcomes compared to more explicit forms of racism (Jones et
al., 2016; Lui and Quezada, 2019). Although we anticipated that the effects of ambiguity might be more profound when the incident involved assault, both Blacks and Whites in the present
study were _less_ likely to appraise violence that was racially ambiguous as constituting discrimination, compared to mild ambiguous discrimination, and minimized the seriousness of the
consequences for the target. In addition, racially ambiguous violence did not elicit the same level of distress or anger as when the violence was explicitly racist. It appears that violence
might serve to discount ambiguous racist cues, such that individuals are more likely to attribute causality to the exceptional malevolence of the perpetrator. Albeit speculative, this may
serve as a defensive strategy that, among Blacks, creates an illusion of control over one’s own risk, or among Whites, minimizes the challenge to their collective moral standing (Leach et
al., 2007). Importantly, the present study was conducted prior to the recent 2020 rise of the Black Lives Matter movement, and it is possible that with the extensive media coverage of the
gratuitous violence perpetrated against Blacks (particularly in the United States), attributions of unjustified violence to racism might be more probable. However, violent intergroup
conflicts have waxed and waned for over a century, and as they do so again, protective psychological dynamics may be restored. At first blush, the results of the present study suggest
considerable similarities in the responses of Black and White Canadians to racism. Both groups were more likely to appraise explicit cues as constituting discrimination, and to report
stronger emotional responses to these events. However, deeper scrutiny suggests that their reactions were also fundamentally different. Specifically, across the four racialized situations
described by the Black target in the videos, Whites were less likely than Blacks to appraise such encounters as discriminatory, were less likely to believe that the events described would
have serious consequences for the target, and reported less distress and anger. These differences could simply be due to a reduced ability to empathize with a cross-race target (Vanman,
2016), or that the events evoked just-world beliefs and by extension a greater likelihood among Whites of blaming the victim (Dawtry et al., 2020). Indeed, although differences in appraisals
depended on whether Whites endorsed modern racist attitudes (with less racist Whites being more likely to acknowledge discrimination and its serious impacts on the target), such attitudes
did not alter emotional distress or anger. The experimental study employed a unique strategy for assessing distress in response to the events in the videos. In particular, rather than
retrospectively assessing distress following the video, which might be heavily influenced by demand characteristics, moment-to-moment ratings were taken as events unfolded (see also Hoggard
et al., 2017). Not only did these ratings reflect greater distress among Blacks, particularly when exposed to explicit racist cues, but these distress ratings were uniquely associated with
higher cortisol levels among Blacks. The relation between reported distress and cortisol levels was not evident among Whites. In addition, the relations between distress and cortisol levels
were stronger among Blacks when the events included physical assault. Earlier research has been inconsistent in documenting cortisol reactions to racism, with some finding blunted responses
to reminder cues of discrimination (Jamieson et al., 2013; Matheson and Anisman, 2012), or a flattening of diurnal levels, including a reduction of the early morning cortisol rise (Zeiders
et al., 2014), whereas others reported elevated levels (Huynh et al., 2017; Matheson et al., 2008). It is possible that specific situational cues may produce a blunted or sensitized cortisol
response to racism, which might further vary depending on the emotional reactions elicited, such as distress or shame (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004; Matheson and Anisman, 2009). As described
earlier, elevated cortisol levels in response to stressors can have multiple adaptive functions that limit adverse psychological and physical health consequences (Anisman et al., 2018). When
stressor conditions are sufficiently severe and occur on a chronic intermittent basis, as is the case with racism, allostatic overload may develop, leading to pathology (McEwen and Akil,
2020). Moreover, the accumulation of sustained social conflicts, such as racism, may lead to ‘type 2’ allostatic overload that requires changes of social structures, rather than individual
interventions to overcome negative consequences (McEwen and Wingfield, 2003). This highlights the relevance of the socio-political context in Canada that, through its apparent celebration of
diversity, may serve to close down challenges regarding ongoing systemic racism (Donovan, 2020). The results of the present investigation provide some insight into the contextual factors
that contribute to how racism is experienced by Blacks and reacted to by Whites. While clarity of the racist motives of the perpetrator facilitates both groups’ ability to call out racism
for what it is, even as Whites may report anger in response to such events, they are unlikely to experience the same depth of response to such injustices. Whites’ motives in response to
racism, and their efforts to understand it, have been found to vary considerably (Radke et al., 2020). Likewise, in the present study individual differences were evident, and indeed, much
like Blacks with a history of racist encounters, Whites who did not endorse modern racist views were more likely to recognize ambiguous cues (when they were mild) as constituting racism, and
to acknowledge the seriousness of racism on the target’s well-being. While acknowledging that Blacks encounter discrimination may be an important first step in alliances to eradicate
systemic racism, much like the undermining implications of Blacks’ beliefs that others hold them in high (public) regard, simply talking about injustice, without explicit and tangible allied
action may do more harm than good. In this regard, national policies of multiculturalism must go beyond the platitudes of celebrating cultural variations to encouraging a strong public
discourse that allows for the acknowledgement of systemic racism. DATA AVAILABILITY The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available in the Scholars Portal
Dataverse repository, https://doi.org/10.5683/SP2/QNBWQP (Study 1) and https://doi.org/10.5683/SP2/YBYX8M (Study 2). NOTES * Black Canadians is the term used to refer to citizens or
permanent residents of Canada who self-define as having full or partial sub-Saharan African descent. While the term African Canadian (paralleling African American) is used occasionally,
Black people of Caribbean origin in Canada reject the term as it does not convey their uniquely Caribbean heritage. People of Colour is viewed as an especially offensive term to some Black
Canadians, as it divides out and lumps together everyone who is non-White, along with its historical echoes of the term ‘coloured people’ (Adam, 2020). REFERENCES * Adam M (2020) Why the
term ‘people of colour’ is offensive to so many. Ottawa Citizen. Available via https://ottawacitizen.com/opinion/adam-why-the-term-people-of-colour-is-offensive-to-so-many. Accessed 26 Dec
2020. * Anisman H, Hayley S, Kusnecov A (2018) The immune system and mental health. Wiley Blackwell, London Google Scholar * Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M et al. (1961) An inventory for
measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychol 4:561–569 Article CAS Google Scholar * Becker JC, Tausch N, Wagner U (2011) Emotional consequences of collective action participation:
differentiating self-directed and outgroup-directed emotions. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 37:587–1598 Article Google Scholar * Bloch KR, Taylor T, Martinez K (2020) Playing the race card: White
injury, White victimhood and the paradox of colour-blind ideology in anti-immigrant discourse. Ethn Racial Stud 43:1130–1148 Article Google Scholar * Bombay A, Matheson K, Anisman H (2010)
Decomposing identity: differential relationships between several aspects of ethnic identity and the negative effects of perceived discrimination among First Nations adults in Canada. Cult
Divers Ethn Minor Psychol 16:507–516 Article Google Scholar * Branscombe NR, Schmitt MT, Harvey RD (1999) Perceiving pervasive discrimination among African Americans: Implications for
group identification and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 77:135–149 Article Google Scholar * Brittian AS, Kim SY, Armenta BE et al. (2015) Do dimensions of ethnic identity mediate the
association between perceived ethnic group discrimination and depressive symptoms? Cult Divers Ethn Minor Psychol 21:41–53 Article Google Scholar * Brondolo E, Thompson S, Brady N et al.
(2005) The relationship of racism to appraisals and coping in a community sample. Ethn Dis 15(4):S5 PubMed Google Scholar * Broudy R, Brondolo E, Coakley V et al. (2007) Perceived ethnic
discrimination in relation to daily moods and negative social interactions. J Behav Med 30(1):31–43 Article PubMed Google Scholar * Busch LY, Pössel P, Valentine JC (2017) Meta-analyses
of cardiovascular reactivity to rumination: a possible mechanism linking depression and hostility to cardiovascular disease. Psychol Bull 143:1378–1394 Article PubMed Google Scholar *
Contrada RJ, Ashmore RD, Gary ML et al. (2001) Measures of ethnicity-related stress: psychometric properties, ethnic group differences, and associations with well-being. J Appl Soc Psychol
31:1775–1820 Article Google Scholar * Dawtry RJ, Callan MJ, Harvey AJ, Gheorghui AI (2020) Victims, vignettes, and videos: meta-analytic and experimental evidence that emotional impact
enhances the derogation of innocent victims. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 24:233–259 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * DeLapp RCT, Williams MT (2019) Preparing for racial
discrimination and moving beyond reactive coping: a systematic review. Curr Psychiatry Res Rev 15(1):58–71 Article Google Scholar * Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME (2004) Acute stressors and
cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull 130:355–391 Article PubMed Google Scholar * Donovan D (2020) A learning moment for Erin
O’Toole and Rex Murphy on racism in Canada. Ottawa Life Mag. Available via https://www.ottawalife.com/article/an-answer-to-rex-murphys-questions-on-racism-in-canada Accessed 28 Dec 2020 *
Dunn J, Nelson JK (2011) Challenging the public denial of racism for a deeper multiculturalism. J Intercult Stud 32:587–602 Article Google Scholar * Ellemers N, Barreto M (2009) Collective
action in modern times: how modern expressions of prejudice prevent collective action. J Soc Iss 65:749–768 Google Scholar * Forrest-Bank SS, Cuellar MJ (2018) The mediating effects of
ethnic identity on the relationships between racial microaggression and psychological well-being. Soc Work Res 42:44–56 Article Google Scholar * Forrest-Bank SS, Jenson JM (2015)
Differences in experiences of racial and ethnic microaggression among Asian, Latino/Hispanic, Black, and White young adults. J Sociol Soc Welf 42:141–161 Google Scholar * Garcia DM, Schmitt
M, Branscombe NR, Ellemers N (2010) Women’s reactions to ingroup members who protest discriminatory treatment: the importance of beliefs about inequality and response appropriateness. Eur J
Soc Psychol 40:733–745 Google Scholar * Giamo LS, Schmitt MT, Outten HR (2012) Perceived discrimination, group identification, and life satisfaction among multiracial people: a test of the
rejection-identification model. Cult Div Ethn Min Psychol 18:319–328 Article Google Scholar * Gill R, Matheson K (2006) Responses to discrimination: the role of emotion and expectations
for emotional regulation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 32:149–161 Article PubMed Google Scholar * Gong F, Xu J, Takeuchi DT (2017) Racial and ethnic differences in perceptions of everyday
discrimination. Sociol Race Ethn 3:506–521 Article Google Scholar * Hayes AF (2017) Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. Guilford Press, New York Google
Scholar * Hoggard LS, Jones S, Sellers RM (2017) Racial cues and racial identity: implications for how African Americans experience and respond to racial discrimination. J Black Psychol
43:409–432 Article Google Scholar * Huynh VW, Huynh QL, Stein MP (2017) Not just sticks and stones: indirect ethnic discrimination leads to greater physiological reactivity. Cult Div Ethn
Min Psychol 23:425–434 Article Google Scholar * Jamieson JP, Koslov K, Nock MK, Mendes WB (2013) Experiencing discrimination increases risk taking. Psychol Sci 24:131–139 Article PubMed
Google Scholar * Jones AM (2020) In Canada, it’s racism with a smile: Donovan Bailey reflects on racial inequality. CTV News. Available via
https://www.ctvnews.ca/sports/in-canada-it-s-racism-with-a-smile-donovan-bailey-reflects-on-racial-inequality-1.4973814 Accessed 27 Dec 2020 * Jones KP, Arena DF, Nittrouer CL et al. (2017)
Subtle discrimination in the workplace: a vicious cycle. Indus Org Psychol 10:51–76 Article Google Scholar * Jones KP, Peddie CI, Gilrane VL et al. (2016) Not so subtle: a meta-analytic
investigation of the correlates of subtle and overt discrimination. J Manag 42:1588–1613 Google Scholar * Karmali F, Kawakami K, Page-Gould E (2017) He said what? Physiological and
cognitive responses to imagining and witnessing outgroup racism. J Exp Psychol 146:1073–1085 Article Google Scholar * Kunstman JW, Tuscherer T, Trawalter S, Lloyd EP (2016) What lies
beneath? Minority group members’ suspicion of Whites’ egalitarian motivation predicts responses to Whites’ smiles. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 42:1193–1205 Article PubMed Google Scholar * Leach
CW, Ellemers N, Barreto M (2007) Group virtue: the importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. J Personal Soc Psychol 93:234–249 Article
Google Scholar * Lilienfeld SO (2017) Microaggressions: strong claims, inadequate evidence. Psychol Sci 12:138–169 Google Scholar * Lui PP (2020) Racial microaggression, overt
discrimination, and distress: (In)direct associations with psychological adjustment. Couns Psychol 48:551–582 Article Google Scholar * Lui PP, Quezada L (2019) Associations between
microaggression and adjustment outcomes: a meta-analytic and narrative review. Psychol Bull 145:45–78 Article PubMed Google Scholar * Lum Z (2020) Systemic racism exists in Canada. It’s a
fact, not an opinion. Huffington Post. Available via https://www.huffingtonpost.ca/entry/systemic-racism-canada_ca_5ed93343c5b69dee016ed31f. Accessed 27 Dec 2020 * Major B, Quinton WJ,
McCoy SK (2002) Antecedents and consequences of attribution to discrimination: theoretical and empirical advances. In: Zanna M (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology, vol 34.
Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 251–330 Google Scholar * Matheson K, Anisman H (2012) Biological and psychosocial responses to discrimination. In: Jetten J, Haslam SA, Haslam C (eds) The
social cure. Psychology Press, London, pp. 133–154 Google Scholar * Matheson K, Anisman H (2009) Anger and shame elicited by discrimination: moderating role of coping on action endorsements
and salivary cortisol. Eur J Soc Psychol 39:163–185 Article Google Scholar * Matheson K, Foster MD, Bombay A et al. (2019) Trauma, perceived discrimination, and distress symptoms in
socially marginalized groups. Front Psychol 10:416 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Matheson K, Jorden S, Anisman H (2008) Relations between trauma experiences and
psychological, physical and neuroendocrine functioning among Somali refugees: mediating role of coping with acculturation stressors. J Immig Min Health 10:291–304 Article Google Scholar *
McConahay JB (1986) Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. In: Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL (eds) Prejudice, discrimination, and racism. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 91–125
Google Scholar * McEwen BS, Akil H (2020) Revisiting the stress concept: implications for affective disorders. J Neurosci 40:12–21 Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar *
McEwen BS, Wingfield JC (2003) The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm Behav 4:2–15 Article Google Scholar * Mercer SH, Zeigler-Hill V, Wallace M, Hayes DM (2011)
Development and initial validation of the Inventory of Microaggressions Against Black Individuals. J Counsel Psychol 58:457–469 Article Google Scholar * Michaud K, Matheson K, Kelly O,
Anisman H (2008) Impact of stressors in a natural context on release of cortisol in healthy adult humans: a meta-analysis. Stress 11:177–197 Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Neuman K
(2019) Race relations in Canada 2019 survey. Environics Institute. Available via https://www.environicsinstitute.org/projects/project-details/race-relations-in-canada-2019. * Operario D,
Fiske S (2001) Ethnic identity moderates perceptions of prejudice: judgments of personal versus group discrimination and subtle versus blatant bias. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 27:550–561 Article
Google Scholar * Passini S (2019) The indifference that makes a difference: why unconcern for minorities disguises prejudicial attitudes. J Moral Educ 48:263–274 Article Google Scholar
* Pieterse A, Powell SA (2016) Theoretical overview of the impact of racism on People of Color. In: Alvarez AN, Liang CTH, Neville HA (eds) Cultural, racial, and ethnic psychology book
series. The cost of racism for People of Color: contextualizing experiences of discrimination. American Psychological Association, Washington, pp. 11–30 Google Scholar * Pieterse AL, Todd
NR, Neville HA, Carter RT (2012) Perceived racism and mental health among Black American adults: a meta-analytic review. J Counsel Psychol 59:1–9 Article Google Scholar * Pittman CT (2011)
Getting mad but ending up sad: the mental health consequences for African Americans using anger to cope with racism. J Black Stud 42:1106–1124 Article PubMed Google Scholar * Pruessner
JC, Kirschbaum C, Meinlschmid G, Hellhammer DH (2003) Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time dependent change.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 28:916–931 Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Quinn DM, Williams MK, Quintana F et al. (2014) Examining effects of anticipated stigma, centrality, salience,
internalization, and outness on psychological distress for people with concealable stigmatized identities. PLoS ONE 9:e96977 Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar *
Radke HRM, Kutlaca M, Siem B et al. (2020) Beyond allyship: motivations for advantaged group members to engage in action for disadvantaged groups. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 24:291–315 Article
PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Ray B, Preston V (2015) Working with diversity: a geographical analysis of ethno-racial discrimination in Toronto. Urban Stud 52:1505–1522 Article
Google Scholar * Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU (2000) How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr
Rev 21:55–89 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schmader T, Croft A, Scarnier M et al. (2012) Implicit and explicit emotional reactions to witnessing prejudice. Group Proc Intergr Rel
15:379–392 Article Google Scholar * Schmitt MT, Branscombe NR, Postmes T, Garcia A (2014) The consequences of perceived discrimination for psychological well-being: a meta-analytic review.
Psychol Bull 140:921–948 Article PubMed Google Scholar * Sellers RM, Copeland‐Linder N, Martin PP, Lewis RH (2006) Racial identity matters: the relationship between racial discrimination
and psychological functioning in African American adolescents. J Res Adol 16:187–216 Article Google Scholar * Sellers RM, Smith M, Shelton JN et al. (1998) Multidimensional model of
racial identity: a reconceptualization of African American racial identity. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2:18–39 Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Siegel JM (1986) The multidimensional anger
inventory. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:191–200 Article ADS PubMed Google Scholar * Sosoo EE, Bernard DL, Neblett Jr EW (2019) The influence of internalized racism on the relationship between
discrimination and anxiety. Cult Div Ethnic Min Psychol 26:570–580 Google Scholar * Statistics Canada (2019). Diversity of Black population in Canada: an overview. Available via
https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/en/pub/89-657-x/89-657-x2019002-eng.pdf?st=j0qEq2yM. Accessed 26 Dec 2020. * Stelkia K (2020) Police brutality in Canada: a symptom of structural racism and
colonial violence. Yellowhead Institute. Available via https://yellowheadinstitute.org/2020/07/15/police-brutality-in-canada-a-symptom-of-structural-racism-and-colonial-violence/. Accessed
27 Dec 2020. * Sue DW (2010) Microaggressions in everyday life: Race, gender, and sexual orientation. Wiley Press, New Jersey Google Scholar * Tao KW, Owen J, Drinane JM (2017) Was _that_
racist? An experimental study of microaggression ambiguity and emotional reactions for racial–ethnic minority and white individuals. Race Soc Probl 9:262–271 Article Google Scholar *
Torres L, Reveles AK, Mata-Greve F et al. (2020) Reactions to witnessing ethnic microaggressions: an experimental study. J Social Clin Psychol 39:141–164 Article Google Scholar * Tougas F,
Brown R, Beaton A, Joly S (1995) Neo-sexism: plus ca change, plus c’est pareil. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 21:842–849 Article Google Scholar * Van Zomeren M, Spears R, Fischer AH, Leach CW
(2004) Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy. J Pers Soc Psychol 87:649–664 Article PubMed Google Scholar
* Vanman EJ (2016) The role of empathy in intergroup relations. Curr Opin Psychol 11:59–63 Article Google Scholar * Zeiders KH, Hoyt LT, Adam EK (2014) Associations between self-reported
discrimination and diurnal cortisol rhythms among young adults: the moderating role of racial-ethnic minority status. Psychoneuroendocrinology 50:280–288 Article CAS PubMed PubMed
Central Google Scholar Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This research was supported by a research grant from the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (grant number #86477). AUTHOR
INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Neuroscience, Carleton University, Ottawa, Canada Kimberly Matheson, Mathew Kent & Hymie Anisman * University of Ottawa Institute of
Mental Health Research, Ottawa, Canada Kimberly Matheson & Hymie Anisman * Department of Psychology, Carleton University, Ottawa, Canada Andrena Pierre * Department of Psychology,
Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Canada Mindi D. Foster Authors * Kimberly Matheson View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Andrena Pierre
View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Mindi D. Foster View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar *
Mathew Kent View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Hymie Anisman View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google
Scholar CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Kimberly Matheson. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The authors declare no competing interests. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER’S NOTE
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit
to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and
your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this
license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Matheson, K., Pierre, A., Foster, M.D. _et al._ Untangling racism:
Stress reactions in response to variations of racism against Black Canadians. _Humanit Soc Sci Commun_ 8, 34 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00711-2 Download citation * Received:
21 July 2020 * Accepted: 11 January 2021 * Published: 03 February 2021 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00711-2 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be
able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing
initiative